[Date Prev][Date Next][Thread Prev][Thread Next][Date Index][Thread Index]
Re: [xmca] Language May Help Create, Not Just Convey, Thoughts and Feelings
- To: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
- Subject: Re: [xmca] Language May Help Create, Not Just Convey, Thoughts and Feelings
- From: Joseph Gilbert <joeg4us@roadrunner.com>
- Date: Fri, 19 Nov 2010 21:04:52 -0800
- Delivered-to: xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
- In-reply-to: <1422829020.1313212.1290190702499.JavaMail.root@mail04.pantherlink.uwm.edu>
- List-archive: <http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/private/xmca>
- List-help: <mailto:xmca-request@weber.ucsd.edu?subject=help>
- List-id: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca.weber.ucsd.edu>
- List-post: <mailto:xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
- List-subscribe: <http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca>, <mailto:xmca-request@weber.ucsd.edu?subject=subscribe>
- List-unsubscribe: <http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca>, <mailto:xmca-request@weber.ucsd.edu?subject=unsubscribe>
- References: <1422829020.1313212.1290190702499.JavaMail.root@mail04.pantherlink.uwm.edu>
- Reply-to: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
- Sender: xmca-bounces@weber.ucsd.edu
Serpel;
I suggest you explore your own emotional/somatic reactions to the
vocal sounds of whatever language you wish. If you do not feel any
reaction, then look for what emotional states the sounds suggest?
Spoken language is founded upon sounds of the body, sounds that
convey states of the body's emotions. The use of these sounds as
words, to refer to things, is based upon their being expressive
emotionally. Scientists have been distracted by the referential
function of spoken language and have neglected to study the primal
function of vocal body language, the conveyance of what's going on
with the moment-by-moment emotional process.
Joseph Gilbert
On Nov 19, 2010, at 10:18 AM, Serpil S Sonmez wrote:
I thought this is a very timely article after Vygotsky/Sapir/Whorf/
Saussure discussions.
Best,
Serpil S. Fox
http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2010/11/101103111206.htm
The paper appears in the Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.
"Charlemagne is reputed to have said that to speak another language
is to possess another soul," says co-author Oludamini Ogunnaike, a
graduate student at Harvard. "This study suggests that language is
much more than a medium for expressing thoughts and feelings. Our
work hints that language creates and shapes our thoughts and
feelings as well."
Implicit attitudes, positive or negative associations people may be
unaware they possess, have been shown to predict behavior towards
members of social groups. Recent research has shown that these
attitudes are quite malleable, susceptible to factors such as the
weather, popular culture -- or, now, by the language people speak.
"Can we shift something as fundamental as what we like and dislike
by changing the language in which our preferences are elicited?"
asks co-author Mahzarin R. Banaji, the Richard Clarke Cabot
Professor of Social Ethics at Harvard. "If the answer is yes, that
gives more support to the idea that language is an important shaper
of attitudes."
Ogunnaike, Banaji, and Yarrow Dunham, now at the University of
California, Merced, used the well-known Implicit Association Test
(IAT), where participants rapidly categorize words that flash on a
computer screen or are played through headphones. The test gives
participants only a fraction of a second to categorize words, not
enough to think about their answers.
"The IAT bypasses a large part of conscious cognition and taps into
something we're not aware of and can't easily control," Banaji says.
The researchers administered the IAT in two different settings:
once in Morocco, with bilinguals in Arabic and French, and again in
the U.S. with Latinos who speak both English and Spanish.
In Morocco, participants who took the IAT in Arabic showed greater
preference for other Moroccans. When they took the test in French,
that difference disappeared. Similarly, in the U.S., participants
who took the test in Spanish showed a greater preference for other
Hispanics. But again, in English, that preference disappeared.
"It was quite shocking to see that a person could take the same
test, within a brief period of time, and show such different
results," Ogunnaike says. "It's like asking your friend if he likes
ice cream in English, and then turning around and asking him again
in French and getting a different answer."
In the Moroccan test, participants saw "Moroccan" names (such as
Hassan or Fatimah) or "French" names (such as Jean or Marie) flash
on a monitor, along with words that are "good" (such as happy or
nice) or "bad" (such as hate or mean). Participants might press one
key when they see a Moroccan name or a good word, and press another
when they see a French name or a bad word. Then the key assignments
are switched so that "Moroccan" and "bad" share the same key and
"French" and "good" share the other.
Linguist Benjamin Lee Whorf first posited in the 1930s that
language is so powerful that it can determine thought. Mainstream
psychology has taken the more skeptical view that while language
may affect thought processes, it doesn't influence thought itself.
This new study suggests that Whorf's idea, when not caricatured,
may generate interesting hypotheses that researchers can continue
to test.
"These results challenge our views of attitudes as stable," Banaji
says. "There still remain big questions about just how fixed or
flexible they are, and language may provide a window through which
we will learn about their nature."
Ogunnaike, Dunham, and Banaji's work was supported by Harvard's
Weatherhead Center for International Affairs and the Mellon Mays
Foundation.
]
__________________________________________
_____
xmca mailing list
xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
__________________________________________
_____
xmca mailing list
xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca