[Date Prev][Date Next][Thread Prev][Thread Next][Date Index][Thread Index]

Re: [xmca] moral life of babies



I think with Larry's additional comment to me and now this great
case, the general point is clear. There is some sort of culturally organized
"imaginary" (yes, prolepsis) in all parent-infant interactions. That is the
kind of "functional equivalent" i was talking about.

Quechua have the manta pouch to keep their babies alive at 12,000 ft
and a very low average daytime temperature. The problem is that I have
only read psychologists' accounts of their actual interactional patterns.
Not clear from those how prolepsis works.

Any idea of the infant mortality rate among the Kogi? Seems like it might be
a little difficult to make it past a couple of months! (Among the Kpelle
when i worked in Liberia it was about 50%, but that owing to high levels of
malaria and nasty water, and of course, no money to buy prophylactic
medicines. Extreme poverty makes for very different imaginaries.
mike



On Sat, May 15, 2010 at 3:10 PM, Martin Packer <packer@duq.edu> wrote:

> On May 12, 2010, at 5:16 PM, mike cole wrote:
>
> > Question: The pattern Martin describes with Sarah and Jenny (hmmm, i once
> > had a little one named Jenny) appears very wide spread among
> > American middle class mothers. It appears virtually absent in a lot of
> > cultures (Kaluli, Yucatec Maya........). Seems doubtful that the Kaluli
> or
> > the Maya lack imagination or communication. What might be the functional
> > equivalents?
> > mike
>
> Mike, you are certainly correct that what I described between Sarah and
> Jenny is culture-specific. But I think Larry is equally correct to say that
> imagination (prolepsis?) is involved in all cultures' treatment of their
> children. I am reading the ethnographies by Gerardo Reichel-Dolmatoff of the
> Kogi people of the Sierra Nevada mountains in the north of Colombia. Life
> there is certainly very different from Northern California! The Kogi
> consider their newborn to be "little adults" that "still know nothing" and
> so are not to be held responsible for their actions. Boy infants are
> considered to be already masculine, a companion to the father and a member
> of the group of adult men. Girls are involved from the outset in the
> economic activity of their mothers. Both genders are from a very young age
> taken by their mother into the mountain as part of daily activity.
>
> For the first 4 weeks the baby is firmly wrapped and carried by the mother,
> along with her other burdens. At first they cry a lot, from hunger, physical
> discomfort, or abrupt changes in temperature, but because the adults can do
> little to satisfy their desires the cries soon cease, and babies older than
> a month rarely cry.
>
> From around a month until 6 months the infant is carried in a cotton bag,
> one of the "mochilas" that is made locally. The mother carries this bag on
> her back, tying its strap in front of her. Only the child's head protrudes,
> flopping from side to side until it happens to rest against the mother's
> back. In the house the bag is hung vertically wherever is convenient, with
> the baby still inside, its back against the wall. This is generally how the
> baby sleeps, though occasionally the mother will take it out of the bag and
> they will sleep together for warmth. This practice, however, is considered
> "dangerous," and there is much talk of occasions when a mother squashed her
> baby (I think 'squashed' is the translation!).
>
> Outside, the baby is hung from a handy tree branch or the bag is placed on
> the ground. The infant can move only its head, but seems not to be
> inconvenienced by the restriction. Often the baby cries only when it is
> removed from the bag. It is generally taken out only to be washed, warmed in
> front of the fire, or fed. Only when, more than 6 months old, the infant
> starts to move so vigorously that it may fall from the bag during the night
> does the mother allow it to sleep with her. Outside the house the baby still
> travels in the bag, but inside it now generally sits, and soon begins to
> crawl. R-D describes this sudden liberty as a new phase in which the infant
> discovers the possibility of moving towards and away from people, and moving
> towards the fire for warmth. The adults find these efforts amusing, and say
> that the infant now "goes searching for food!" They express their pride and
> approval, and consider that now the infant is definitely entering the
> "human" phase. This is the occasion for a small ceremony to which the shaman
> is invited, and at which plantain, yuca, beans and meat are served. The
> shaman performs a chant in which he asks the universal mother (the earth) to
> care for the child its whole life. This is, R-D proposes, a kind of
> initiation rite into the adult cycle of nutrition which has much importance
> for the individual.
>
> Here there seems to be much less face-to-face interaction than is typical
> in the US or England, but still adults are interpreting the child's
> motivation in what Larry refers to as an "as if" way, so that the infant is
> completely embedded in a cultural and historical matrix - a second, social,
> womb from which a recognizable Kogi eventually emerges. R-D's interpretation
> of the mechanisms is too Freudian for my taste, but even so he is surely
> correct to see the origins of adult personality and worldview in these
> practices of child-rearing.
>
> Martin
>
>
>
_______________________________________________
xmca mailing list
xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca