[Date Prev][Date Next][Thread Prev][Thread Next][Date Index][Thread Index]
Re: [xmca] Can the Right Kinds of Play Teach Self-Control?
- To: Ageliki Nicolopoulou <agn3@lehigh.edu>
- Subject: Re: [xmca] Can the Right Kinds of Play Teach Self-Control?
- From: mike cole <lchcmike@gmail.com>
- Date: Sun, 27 Sep 2009 09:52:00 -0700
- Cc: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
- Delivered-to: xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
- Dkim-signature: v=1; a=rsa-sha256; c=relaxed/relaxed; d=gmail.com; s=gamma; h=domainkey-signature:mime-version:received:reply-to:in-reply-to :references:date:message-id:subject:from:to:cc:content-type; bh=eunRjQftmnwKDUsdiRZWzVHyqyf2ioSpyxLR42/wrK4=; b=ZkBxnBwg85g6n6qfXkBvIVEbx9IkjDs2EK/qiIgtO2NkLhy0wlZ0kiAf10WxJ5626f 1ItJf/S6kKV1iIo4Mc1d/WLAqfWXCDtJjcX8yALaiD8XZGrxc/aSiEs65bf4cXrrxFhm gPS9IL+wWc2JJYRHujSwsxdPuJ7SknPFvbiVU=
- Domainkey-signature: a=rsa-sha1; c=nofws; d=gmail.com; s=gamma; h=mime-version:reply-to:in-reply-to:references:date:message-id :subject:from:to:cc:content-type; b=E4VbuQkVscI5njIjeJjDlR6t1JTyngdKoOxK0msg22UicxaXWgUGRyqIfCHzDBkIi7 grupl1mgfUoQNAyqI2G7D96sUUHhMLv0U+qWBA+Xe5HuysgBmr5jY38Dhoe6GNcrbOz6 Vt8o1cd4bOHe+JcrQCy2Z3ylq+2hRxDMVtl4g=
- In-reply-to: <4ABF9717.4010706@lehigh.edu>
- List-archive: <http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/private/xmca>
- List-help: <mailto:xmca-request@weber.ucsd.edu?subject=help>
- List-id: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca.weber.ucsd.edu>
- List-post: <mailto:xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
- List-subscribe: <http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca>, <mailto:xmca-request@weber.ucsd.edu?subject=subscribe>
- List-unsubscribe: <http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca>, <mailto:xmca-request@weber.ucsd.edu?subject=unsubscribe>
- References: <004101ca3f7f$e6d7a890$b486f9b0$@edu> <30364f990909270830y287bccfbqfb16a9aa9474e09d@mail.gmail.com> <4ABF8F9A.7070308@lehigh.edu> <30364f990909270922g3227c578wf92d4026dcd8c98d@mail.gmail.com> <4ABF9717.4010706@lehigh.edu>
- Reply-to: lchcmike@gmail.com, "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
- Sender: xmca-bounces@weber.ucsd.edu
Everyone re-frames to suit themselves, and hopes that it sticks. We stand on
the backs of giants by climbing up on them, unbidden, and shouting from a
higher position... until the giant disintegrates, either vanguished by some
new giant or nibbled to death by ducks.
I do not know about you, but I am running many messages behind the
discussion.
mike
On Sun, Sep 27, 2009 at 9:47 AM, Ageliki Nicolopoulou <agn3@lehigh.edu>wrote:
> Thanks, Mike. I can now see why you see it in terms of re-framing. I just
> wasn't sure which side you were seeing as re-framing the issues. (And yes to
> some degree I have followed the Learning Sciences and Vygotsky
> discussion).--Ageliki
>
> mike cole wrote:
>
>> I am pushed to get ready for classes monday, Ageliki.
>> I would be glad to discuss the issue I referred to as re-framing within
>> the
>> context of the discussion of learning sciences and vygotsky just to keep
>> it in the bounds of time constraints-- have you read that discussion?
>> Otherwise my comments will make no sense.
>>
>> Within that context, I might start with executive functioning as a
>> "neuroscience term," the discourse on 0-3 and ways to make babies brains
>> develop more quickly (see xmca discussion of brain and education),and the
>> linkages to no-child-left behind. Seems a long way from Kharkov in the late
>> 1930's, or 1990's, or the recent (to the NYTimes) discovery of Vygotsky.
>>
>> mike
>> Sun, Sep 27, 2009 at 9:15 AM, Ageliki Nicolopoulou <agn3@lehigh.edu<mailto:
>> agn3@lehigh.edu>> wrote:
>>
>> Hi Mike,
>>
>> Can you explain a bit what you mean by re-framing and why you see
>> it as an issue of re-framing?
>>
>> Thanks,
>> Ageliki
>>
>> -- **********************************************
>> Ageliki Nicolopoulou
>> Professor
>> Department of Psychology, Lehigh University
>> 17 Memorial Drive East
>> Bethlehem, PA 18015-3068
>>
>> Personal Webpage: http://www.lehigh.edu/~agn3/index.htm<http://www.lehigh.edu/%7Eagn3/index.htm>
>> <http://www.lehigh.edu/%7Eagn3/index.htm>
>> Departmental Webpage:
>> http://www.lehigh.edu/~inpsy/nicolopoulou.html<http://www.lehigh.edu/%7Einpsy/nicolopoulou.html>
>> <http://www.lehigh.edu/%7Einpsy/nicolopoulou.html>
>> **********************************************
>>
>>
>> mike cole wrote:
>>
>> Thanks Peter-- I was just about to forward this story. Apart
>> from its
>> considerable intrinsic interest to members of this group, it
>> seems relevant
>> to the prior discussion the origins of learning sciences and
>> the ways in
>> which re-framing can operate to change the terms of discourse.
>> mike
>> On Sun, Sep 27, 2009 at 7:36 AM, Peter Smagorinsky
>> <smago@uga.edu <mailto:smago@uga.edu>> wrote:
>>
>>
>> September 27, 2009 The NY Times Magazine Section
>>
>> The School Issue: Preschool
>>
>>
>> Can the Right Kinds of Play Teach Self-Control?
>>
>>
>> By PAUL TOUGH
>>
>>
>>
>> "Come on, Abigail."
>>
>> "No, wait!" Abigail said. "I'm not finished!" She was bent
>> low over her
>> clipboard, a stubby pencil in her hand, slowly scratching
>> out the letters
>> in
>> the book's title, one by one: T H E. . . .
>>
>> "Abigail, we're waiting!" Jocelyn said, staring forcefully
>> at her
>> classmate.
>> Henry, sitting next to her, sighed dramatically.
>>
>> "I'm going as fast as I can!" Abigail said, looking
>> harried. She brushed a
>> strand of hair out of her eyes and plowed ahead: V E R Y.
>> . . .
>>
>> The three children were seated at their classroom's
>> listening center, where
>> their assignment was to leaf through a book together while
>> listening on
>> headphones to a CD with the voice of a teacher reading it
>> aloud. The book
>> in
>> question was lying on the table in front of Jocelyn, and
>> every few seconds,
>> Abigail would jump up and lean over Jocelyn to peer at the
>> cover, checking
>> what came next in the title. Then she would dive back to
>> the paper on her
>> clipboard, and her pencil would carefully shape yet
>> another letter: H U N.
>> .
>> . .
>>
>> Henry fiddled with the CD player. Like Abigail and
>> Jocelyn, he was a
>> kindergarten
>> <
>>
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/subjects/e/education_pr
>> eschool/index.html?inline=nyt-classifier<
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/subjects/e/education_pr%0Aeschool/index.html?inline=nyt-classifier
>> >>
>>
>>
>> student in Red Bank, a small town
>> near the New Jersey shore. The students at the elementary
>> school came
>> mostly
>> from working-class and low-income families, and, like the
>> town itself, the
>> student population was increasingly Hispanic. Jocelyn,
>> with flowing dark
>> hair, was the child of immigrants from Mexico; Henry was
>> Hispanic with a
>> spiky haircut; Abigail was white and blond.
>>
>> "Abby!" Henry said. "Come on!" He and Jocelyn had long ago
>> finished writing
>> the title of the book on their lesson plans. They already
>> had their
>> headphones on. The only thing standing between them and
>> the story was the
>> pencil clutched in their classmate's hand.
>>
>> G R Y. . . .
>>
>> "O.K., we're starting," Jocelyn announced. But they didn't
>> start. For all
>> their impatience, they knew the rule of the listening
>> center: You don't
>> start listening to the story until everyone is ready.
>>
>> "Oh, man," Henry said. He grabbed his face and lowered his
>> head to the desk
>> with a clunk.
>>
>> C A T E R. . . .
>>
>> "Let's begin!" Jocelyn said.
>>
>> "I'm almost done!" Abigail was hopping up and down now.
>> "Don't press it!"
>> She bounced from foot to foot, still writing: P I L. . . .
>>
>> "I'm pressing it!" Henry said. His finger hovered over the
>> play button on
>> the CD player . . . but it did not fall, not until Abigail
>> etched out her
>> last few letters and put on her headphones. Only then,
>> finally, could the
>> three of them turn the pages together and listen to "The
>> Very Hungry
>> Caterpillar."
>>
>> When the CD finished, each child took a piece of paper and
>> drew three
>> pictures to illustrate what happened at the beginning, in
>> the middle and at
>> the end of the book. Then they captioned each one, first
>> drawing a series
>> of
>> horizontal lines under the pictures, one for each word,
>> and then writing
>> out
>> each word, or an approximation thereof: For "butterfly,"
>> Abigail wrote
>> "btrfli." Their language skills were pretty impressive for
>> kindergarten
>> students. But for the teachers and child psychologists
>> running the program
>> in which they were enrolled, those skills were considered
>> secondary - not
>> irrelevant, but not as important as the skills the
>> children displayed
>> before
>> the story started, when all three were wrestling with
>> themselves, fighting
>> to overcome their impulses - in Abby's case, the
>> temptation to give up on
>> writing out the whole title and just submit to the pleas
>> of her friends;
>> for
>> Jocelyn and Henry, the urge to rip the pencil out of
>> Abby's hand and start
>> the CD already.
>>
>> Over the last few years, a new buzz phrase has emerged
>> among scholars and
>> scientists who study early-childhood development, a phrase
>> that sounds more
>> as if it belongs in the boardroom than the classroom:
>> executive function.
>> Originally a neuroscience term, it refers to the ability
>> to think straight:
>> to order your thoughts, to process information in a
>> coherent way, to hold
>> relevant details in your short-term memory, to avoid
>> distractions and
>> mental
>> traps and focus on the task in front of you. And recently,
>> cognitive
>> psychologists have come to believe that executive
>> function, and
>> specifically
>> the skill of self-regulation, might hold the answers to
>> some of the most
>> vexing questions in education today.
>>
>> The ability of young children to control their emotional
>> and cognitive
>> impulses, it turns out, is a remarkably strong indicator
>> of both short-term
>> and long-term success, academic and otherwise. In some
>> studies,
>> self-regulation skills have been shown to predict academic
>> achievement more
>> reliably than I.Q. tests. The problem is that just as
>> we're coming to
>> understand the importance of self-regulation skills, those
>> skills appear to
>> be in short supply among young American children. In one
>> recent national
>> survey, 46 percent of kindergarten teachers said that at
>> least half the
>> kids
>> in their classes had problems following directions. In
>> another study, Head
>> Start teachers reported that more than a quarter of their
>> students
>> exhibited
>> serious self-control-related negative behaviors, like
>> kicking or
>> threatening
>> other students, at least once a week. Walter Gilliam, a
>> professor at Yale
>> <
>>
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/organizations/y/yale_un
>> iversity/index.html?inline=nyt-org<
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/organizations/y/yale_un%0Aiversity/index.html?inline=nyt-org
>> >>
>>
>>
>> 's child-study center, estimates that
>> each year, across the country, more than 5,000 children
>> are expelled from
>> pre-K programs because teachers feel unable to control them.
>>
>> There is a popular belief that executive-function skills
>> are fixed early
>> on,
>> a function of genes and parenting, and that other than
>> medication, there's
>> not much that teachers and professionals can do to affect
>> children's
>> impulsive behavior. In fact, though, there is growing
>> evidence that the
>> opposite is true, that executive-function skills are
>> relatively malleable -
>> quite possibly more malleable than I.Q., which is
>> notoriously hard to
>> increase over a sustained period. In laboratory studies,
>> research
>> psychologists have found that with executive function,
>> practice helps; when
>> children or adults repeatedly perform basic exercises in
>> cognitive
>> self-regulation, they get better at it. But when
>> researchers try to take
>> those experiments out of the lab and into the classroom,
>> their success rate
>> is much lower. Angela Duckworth, a psychologist at the
>> University of
>> <
>>
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/organizations/u/univers
>> ity_of_pennsylvania/index.html?inline=nyt-org<
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/organizations/u/univers%0Aity_of_pennsylvania/index.html?inline=nyt-org
>> >>
>>
>>
>> Pennsylvania, has spent the
>> last seven years trying to find reliable, repeatable
>> methods to improve
>> self-control in children. When I spoke to her recently,
>> she told me about a
>> six-week-long experiment that she and some colleagues
>> conducted in 2003
>> with
>> 40 fifth-grade students at a school in Philadelphia.
>>
>> "We did everything right," she told me: led the kids
>> through self-control
>> exercises, helped them reorganize their lockers, gave them
>> rewards for
>> completing their homework. And at the end of the
>> experiment, the students
>> dutifully reported that they now had more self-control
>> than when they
>> started the program. But in fact, they did not: the
>> children who had been
>> through the intervention did no better on a variety of
>> measures than a
>> control group at the same school. "We looked at teacher
>> ratings of
>> self-control, we looked at homework completion, we looked
>> at standardized
>> achievement tests, we looked at G.P.A., we looked at
>> whether they were late
>> to class more," Duckworth explained. "We got zero effect
>> on everything."
>> Despite that failure, Duckworth says she is convinced that
>> it is possible
>> to
>> boost executive function among children - she just thinks
>> it will require a
>> more complex and thoroughgoing program than the one that
>> she and her
>> colleagues employed. "It's not impossible," she concludes,
>> "but it's damn
>> hard."
>>
>> Which is why Abigail, Henry and Jocelyn are potentially so
>> important. They
>> and their classmates are enrolled in Tools of the Mind, a
>> relatively new
>> program dedicated to improving the self-regulation
>> abilities of young
>> children, starting as early as age 3. Tools of the Mind is
>> based on the
>> teachings of Lev Vygotsky, a Russian psychologist who died
>> of tuberculosis
>> in 1934, at age 38, and whose educational theories and
>> methods were, until
>> recently, little known in the United States. Over the past
>> 15 years,
>> Deborah
>> Leong and Elena Bodrova, scholars of child development
>> based in Denver,
>> have
>> turned Vygotsky's philosophy into a full-time curriculum for
>> prekindergarten
>> and kindergarten students, complete with training manuals
>> and coaches and
>> professional-development classes for teachers. Tools of
>> the Mind has grown
>> steadily - though its expansion has sped up in the past
>> few years - and it
>> now is being used to teach 18,000 prekindergarten and
>> kindergarten students
>> in 12 states around the country. Leong and Bodrova say
>> they believe they
>> have found the answer to the problem that has bedeviled
>> Duckworth and other
>> psychologists for so long. Their program, they say, can
>> reliably teach
>> self-regulation skills to pretty much any child - poor or
>> rich; typical
>> achievers as well as many of those who are considered to
>> have special
>> needs.
>> (They make the claim that many kids given diagnoses of
>> A.D.H.D. would not
>> need Ritalin
>> <
>>
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/news/health/diseasesconditionsandhealthtopics
>> /ritalin_drug/index.html?inline=nyt-classifier<
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/news/health/diseasesconditionsandhealthtopics%0A/ritalin_drug/index.html?inline=nyt-classifier
>> >>
>>
>>
>> if they were enrolled in
>> Tools of the Mind.) And if Leong and Bodrova are right,
>> those improved
>> self-regulation skills will lead not only to fewer
>> classroom meltdowns and
>> expulsions in prekindergarten and kindergarten; they will
>> also lead to
>> better reading and math scores later on.
>>
>> At the heart of the Tools of the Mind methodology is a
>> simple but
>> surprising
>> idea: that the key to developing self-regulation is play,
>> and lots of it.
>> But not just any play. The necessary ingredient is what
>> Leong and Bodrova
>> call "mature dramatic play": complex, extended
>> make-believe scenarios,
>> involving multiple children and lasting for hours, even
>> days. If you want
>> to
>> succeed in school and in life, they say, you first need to
>> do what Abigail
>> and Jocelyn and Henry have done every school day for the
>> past two years:
>> spend hour after hour dressing up in firefighter hats and
>> wedding gowns,
>> cooking make-believe hamburgers and pouring nonexistent
>> tea, doing the
>> hard,
>> serious work of playing pretend.
>>
>> Over the last decade or so, the central debate in the field of
>> early-childhood education has been between one group that
>> favors what you
>> might call a preacademic approach to prekindergarten and
>> kindergarten and
>> another group that contends that the point of school in
>> those early years
>> is
>> not to prepare for academic study; it is to allow children
>> to explore the
>> world, learn social skills and have free, unconstrained
>> fun. The
>> preacademic
>> camp began to dominate the debate in the late 1990s,
>> drawing on some
>> emerging research that showed that children's abilities at
>> the beginning of
>> kindergarten were powerful predictors of later success. If
>> a child reached
>> his 5th birthday well behind his peers in measures of
>> cognitive ability,
>> this research showed, he would most likely never catch up.
>> The good news in
>> the research was that if you exposed struggling children
>> to certain
>> intensive reading and math interventions in
>> prekindergarten and
>> kindergarten, when their minds were still at their most
>> pliable, you could
>> significantly reduce or even eliminate that lag. And so
>> the answer, to many
>> scholars and policy makers, was clear: there was no time
>> to waste in those
>> early years on Play-Doh and fingerpainting, not when kids,
>> and especially
>> disadvantaged kids, could be making such rapid advances in
>> the critical
>> cognitive skills they needed.
>>
>> More recently, though, a backlash has been growing against
>> the preacademic
>> approach among educators and child psychologists who argue
>> that it misses
>> the whole point of early-childhood education.
>> "Kindergarten has ceased to
>> be
>> a garden of delight and has become a place of stress and
>> distress," warned
>> a
>> report released in March by a research group called the
>> Alliance for
>> Childhood, which is advised by some of the country's most
>> esteemed
>> progressive-education scholars. There is now too much
>> testing and too
>> little
>> free time, the report argues, and kids are being forced to
>> try to read
>> before they are ready. The solution, according to the
>> report's authors, is
>> a
>> return to ample doses of "unstructured play" in
>> kindergarten. If kids are
>> allowed to develop at their own paces, they will be
>> happier and healthier
>> and less stressed out. And there will still be plenty of
>> time later on to
>> learn how to read.
>>
>> On the surface, Bodrova and Leong would seem to belong to
>> the second camp.
>> They say, after all, that play should have a central place in
>> early-childhood classrooms. And they do find fault with
>> the academic
>> approach, arguing that in practice, many of the
>> early-childhood academic
>> initiatives that have been introduced in the No Child Left
>> Behind
>> <
>>
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/subjects/n/no_child_lef
>> t_behind_act/index.html?inline=nyt-classifier<
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/subjects/n/no_child_lef%0At_behind_act/index.html?inline=nyt-classifier
>> >>
>>
>>
>> era have failed to produce
>> any significant improvement in academic skills. At the
>> same time, they
>> don't
>> agree that the solution is unstructured free play. The
>> romantic idea that
>> children are born with flowering imaginations and a
>> natural instinct for
>> make-believe is simply wrong, they say. Especially these
>> days, they
>> contend,
>> when children spend more time in front of screens and less
>> time in
>> unsupervised play, kids need careful adult guidance and
>> instruction before
>> they are able to play in a productive way.
>>
>> Bodrova and Leong began working together with
>> early-childhood teachers in
>> 1992, soon after Bodrova immigrated from Russia to be a
>> visiting professor
>> at Metropolitan State College of Denver, where Leong was a
>> professor of
>> child development. When they visited local classrooms,
>> they were struck by
>> how out of control things often seemed. It was a period
>> when preschool and
>> kindergarten teachers were taught to "follow the child's
>> lead," to let
>> children guide the learning process with their own
>> interests and unfettered
>> imaginations. In practice, Bodrova and Leong observed,
>> classrooms were
>> often
>> chaotic free-for-alls.
>>
>> Bodrova and Leong had both studied Vygotsky, and they
>> discussed whether
>> some
>> of his methods might help improve the climate of these
>> classrooms. For
>> Vygotsky, the real purpose of early-childhood education
>> was not to learn
>> content, like the letters of the alphabet or the names of
>> shapes and colors
>> and animals. The point was to learn how to think. When
>> children enter
>> preschool, Vygotsky wrote, they are "slaves to their
>> environment," unable
>> to
>> control their reactions or direct their interests,
>> responding to whatever
>> shiny objects are put in front of them. Accordingly, the
>> most important
>> goal
>> of prekindergarten is to teach children how to master
>> their thoughts. And
>> the best way for children to do that, Vygotsky believed,
>> especially at this
>> early age, is to employ various tools, tricks and habits
>> that train the
>> mind
>> to work at a higher level. So Tools of the Mind students
>> learn to use
>> "private speech" - to talk to themselves as they do a
>> difficult task (like,
>> say, forming the letter W), to help themselves remember
>> what step comes
>> next
>> (down, up, down, up). They use "mediators": physical
>> objects that remind
>> them how to do a particular task, like CD-size cards, one
>> with a pair of
>> lips and one with an ear, that signify whose turn it is to
>> read aloud in
>> Buddy Reading and whose turn it is to listen. But more
>> than anything, they
>> use play.
>>
>> Most of Vygotsky's counterparts in the field of child
>> psychology, including
>> influential figures like Jean Piaget and Maria Montessori,
>> held that
>> imaginary play was an immature form of expression, a
>> preliminary stage of
>> development. But Vygotsky maintained that at 4 or 5, a
>> child's ability to
>> play creatively with other children was in fact a better
>> gauge of her
>> future
>> academic success than any other indicator, including her
>> vocabulary, her
>> counting skills or her knowledge of the alphabet. Dramatic
>> play, he said,
>> was the training ground where children learned to regulate
>> themselves, to
>> conquer their own unruly minds. In the United States, we
>> often associate
>> play with freedom, but to Vygotsky, dramatic play was
>> actually the arena
>> where children's actions were most tightly restricted.
>> When a young boy is
>> acting out the role of a daddy making breakfast, he is
>> limited by all the
>> rules of daddy-ness. Some of those limitations come from
>> his playmates: if
>> he starts acting like a baby (or a policeman or a
>> dinosaur) in the middle
>> of
>> making breakfast, the other children will be sure to steer
>> him back to the
>> eggs and bacon. But even beyond that explicit peer
>> pressure, Vygotsky would
>> say, the child is guided by the basic principles of play.
>> Make-believe
>> isn't
>> as stimulating and satisfying - it simply isn't as much
>> fun - if you don't
>> stick to your role. And when children follow the rules of
>> make-believe and
>> push one another to follow those rules, he said, they
>> develop important
>> habits of self-control.
>>
>> Bodrova and Leong drew on research conducted by some of
>> Vygotsky's
>> followers
>> that showed that children acting out a dramatic scene can
>> control their
>> impulses much better than they can in nonplay situations.
>> In one
>> experiment,
>> 4-year-old children were first asked to stand still for as
>> long as they
>> could. They typically did not make it past a minute. But
>> when the kids
>> played a make-believe game in which they were guards at a
>> factory, they
>> were
>> able to stand at attention for more than four minutes. In
>> another
>> experiment, prekindergarten-age children were asked to
>> memorize a list of
>> unrelated words. Then they played "grocery store" and were
>> asked to
>> memorize
>> a similar list of words - this time, though, as a shopping
>> list. In the
>> play
>> situation, on average, the children were able to remember
>> twice as many
>> words. Bodrova and Leong say they see the same effect in
>> Tools of the Mind
>> classrooms: when their students spend more time on
>> dramatic play, not only
>> does their level of self-control improve, but so do their
>> language skills.
>>
>> In the past, when psychologists (or parents or teachers or
>> priests) tried
>> to
>> improve children's self-control, they used the principles
>> of behaviorism,
>> reinforcing good and bad behaviors with rewards and
>> punishments. The
>> message
>> to kids was that terrible things would happen if they
>> didn't control their
>> impulses, and the role of adults, whether parents or
>> preschool teachers,
>> was
>> to train children by praising them for their positive
>> self-control ("Look
>> at
>> how well Cindy is sitting!") and criticizing them for
>> their lapses. And in
>> most American prekindergartens and kindergartens,
>> behaviorism, in some
>> form,
>> is still the dominant method. But Bodrova and Leong say
>> that those
>> "external
>> reinforcement systems" create "other-directed regulation"
>> - good behavior
>> done not from some internal sense of control but for the
>> approval of
>> others,
>> to avoid punishment and win praise and treats. And that,
>> they say, is a
>> kind
>> of regulation that is not particularly valuable or
>> lasting. Children learn
>> only how to be obedient, how to follow orders, not how to
>> understand and
>> regulate their own impulses. The ultimate goal of Tools of
>> the Mind is not
>> emotional or physical self-regulation; it is cognitive
>> self-regulation -
>> not
>> the ability to avoid grabbing a toy from the kid next to
>> you (though that's
>> an important first step), but the much more subtle ability
>> to avoid falling
>> for a deceptively attractive wrong answer on a test or to
>> concentrate on an
>> arduous mental task. And those abilities are more
>> difficult to affect by
>> other-directed regulation. Because the abilities are more
>> abstract, they
>> are
>> less likely to be elicited by rewards. Kids are rarely
>> able to organize
>> their thoughts better in order to get an ice-cream cone.
>>
>> As a result, many practices that most prekindergarten
>> teachers consider
>> essential are more or less banned from Tools of the Mind
>> classrooms. There
>> are no gold stars, no telling the class that they are all
>> going to have to
>> wait until Jimmy is quiet; even timeouts are discouraged.
>> When there is a
>> conflict - when, say, Billy grabs a toy from Jamal - the
>> Tools of the Mind
>> teacher's first questions are supposed to be: What was it
>> in the classroom
>> that made it hard for Billy to control himself? And what
>> mediators could
>> help him do better next time? The teacher does remind
>> Billy that there is a
>> rule and he broke it, but she doesn't make a big deal out
>> of the incident.
>> "We pretty much try not to use this whole concept of
>> misbehavior," Bodrova
>> told me. "These kids are not born criminals. Even if they
>> do something that
>> is completely out of bounds, they do it because they can't
>> stop
>> themselves."
>>
>> There are not yet firm experimental data that prove that
>> Tools of the Mind
>> works. But two early studies that began in the late 1990s
>> in Denver showed
>> some promising results: After a year in the program,
>> students did
>> significantly better than a similar group on basic
>> measures of literacy
>> ability. And more recent studies, including one overseen
>> by Adele Diamond,
>> a
>> professor at the University of British Columbia who is one
>> of the most
>> prominent researchers in the field of cognitive
>> self-control, have shown
>> that Tools students consistently score higher on tests
>> requiring executive
>> function. Angela Duckworth told me that when she read
>> Diamond's report,
>> which was published in Science in 2007, "I got very
>> excited." Her failed
>> 2003 study had persuaded her that the usual approach to
>> self-control in
>> early-childhood education, a brief intervention here or
>> there, wouldn't
>> work. But Tools of the Mind was clearly a different
>> strategy. "It's an
>> immersion approach," she said. "It's not that these kids
>> are pulled out and
>> they do self-control for half an hour a day. Everything is
>> about
>> self-regulation, every single moment. Everything about the
>> culture that the
>> classroom creates reinforces that."
>>
>> It's one of the reasons that visiting a Tools of the Mind
>> classroom can
>> cause moments of cognitive dissonance. While there's a lot
>> of dressing up
>> and playing with blocks, plenty of messing around with
>> sand tables and
>> Legos
>> and jigsaw puzzles, there are also a few activities that
>> seem not just
>> grown-up but protocorporate, borrowed directly from the
>> modern office.
>> Every
>> morning, before embarking on the day's make-believe play,
>> each child takes
>> a
>> colored marker and a printed form called a play plan and
>> draws or writes
>> his
>> declaration of intent for that day's play: "I am going to
>> drive the
>> choo-choo train"; "I am going to make a sand castle"; "I
>> am going to take
>> the dollies to the beach." At the beginning of
>> prekindergarten, children
>> are
>> coached on dramatic play - called Make-Believe Play
>> Practice - with the
>> teacher leading the children, step by step, through the
>> mechanics of
>> pretending. (The training manual describes how a teacher
>> might coach a
>> child
>> to feed a baby doll: "I'm pretending my baby is crying. Is
>> yours? What
>> should we say?") In kindergarten, every student carries
>> around a clipboard
>> with the day's activities on it - that's what Abigail was
>> writing on at the
>> listening center - and each Friday, every child has a 5-
>> or 10-minute
>> "learning conference" with his teacher, a mini-performance
>> review in which
>> the children discuss what they accomplished in the last
>> week, where they
>> fell short and what skills they want to work on in the
>> week to come. All of
>> these practices, along with plenty of others that fill the
>> day, are
>> designed
>> to reinforce habits of self-control.
>>
>> This comprehensiveness creates an extra level of
>> complication for
>> researchers examining Tools of the Mind. There are now
>> four separate
>> large-scale long-term experimental studies under way
>> across the country.
>> But
>> even if the researchers do find, in a few years, that the
>> program has
>> long-term effects on executive function and school
>> performance, they still
>> won't know exactly which techniques in the Tools of the
>> Mind package are
>> the
>> most useful, or whether they all need to be employed in
>> concert in order to
>> have an effect. Stephanie M. Carlson, a professor of child
>> psychology at
>> the
>> University of Minnesota
>> <
>>
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/organizations/u/univers
>> ity_of_minnesota/index.html?inline=nyt-org<
>> http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/organizations/u/univers%0Aity_of_minnesota/index.html?inline=nyt-org
>> >>
>>
>>
>> who studies executive function,
>> told me she is impressed with what she has seen so far of
>> Tools of the
>> Mind.
>> But, she pointed out, "it's a really heavy-hitting
>> approach, and there are
>> a
>> lot of different techniques used during the course of the
>> day. What we
>> don't
>> know is what the secret ingredient is." It might be all
>> the dramatic play,
>> but it also might be the literacy practice, or the
>> learning conferences, or
>> something else entirely.
>>
>> In the end, the most lasting effect of the Tools of the
>> Mind studies may be
>> to challenge some of our basic ideas about the boundary
>> between work and
>> play. Today, play is seen by most teachers and education
>> scholars as a
>> break
>> from hard work or a reward for positive behaviors, not a
>> place to work on
>> cognitive skills. But in Tools of the Mind classrooms,
>> that distinction
>> disappears: work looks a lot like play, and play is
>> treated more like work.
>> When I asked Duckworth about this, she said it went to the
>> heart of what
>> was
>> new and potentially important about the program. "We often
>> think about play
>> as relaxing and doing what you want to do," she explained.
>> "Maybe it's an
>> American thing: We work really hard, and then we go on
>> vacation and have
>> fun. But in fact, very few truly pleasurable moments come
>> from complete
>> hedonism. What Tools does - and maybe what we all need to
>> do - is to blur
>> the line a bit between what is work and what is play. Just
>> because
>> something
>> is effortful and difficult and involves some amount of
>> constraint doesn't
>> mean it can't be fun."
>>
>> _______________________________________________
>> xmca mailing list
>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu <mailto:xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>>
>>
>> _______________________________________________
>> xmca mailing list
>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu <mailto:xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>>
>>
>>
>>
>>
> --
>
> **********************************************
>
> Ageliki Nicolopoulou
>
> Professor
>
> Department of Psychology, Lehigh University
>
> 17 Memorial Drive East
>
> Bethlehem, PA 18015-3068
>
>
>
> Personal Webpage: http://www.lehigh.edu/~agn3/index.htm<http://www.lehigh.edu/%7Eagn3/index.htm>
>
> Departmental Webpage: http://www.lehigh.edu/~inpsy/nicolopoulou.html<http://www.lehigh.edu/%7Einpsy/nicolopoulou.html>
>
> **********************************************
>
>
>
>
_______________________________________________
xmca mailing list
xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca