[Date Prev][Date Next][Thread Prev][Thread Next][Date Index][Thread Index]

RE: [xmca] Where and What Is A Boundary?( is it betweenmicrogenesis and ontogenesis?)



Dear Michael and everyone. A double bind is primarily within the subjects
experiencing and a contradiction is between nodules of the system. What you
are referring  below to is a contradiction of use value and exchange value
(classical Marxism) and I can easily state that this is not only a dilemma
in the states but in general as education looses sight of its reason to be
world wide.

 

I am a bit astonished that this paper was chosen it evidently lacks
coherence and misinterprets theory. 

 

I know that I said that I was leaving I am don't worry in a few hours but
these discussions are like candyJ

 

Denise

 

From: xmca-bounces@weber.ucsd.edu [mailto:xmca-bounces@weber.ucsd.edu] On
Behalf Of Michael Glassman
Sent: 15 September 2010 23:20
To: eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity
Subject: RE: [xmca] Where and What Is A Boundary?( is it betweenmicrogenesis
and ontogenesis?)

 

Peter, David, Larry, Denise, Mike and whoever,

 

I figured this was a good opportunity to try and understand the learning by
expanding model in the context of the target article and the interesting
Educational Review article.  I have some interrelated questions.  I guess my
first one is because I have been reading Bateson as of late.  Engenstrom
suggests I think it is his second step of the learning by expansion process
that there is a contradiction but also refers to Bateson's double bind.  But
Bateson's double bind goes pretty far beyond dialectical negation.  He
introduced the concept in an article on schizophrenia suggesting that the
natural outcome of a double bind in the family context is basically
insanity.  That is if a child in a family is considered the scapegoat for
the family while at the same time seeing the self as the protector of the
family structure and unable to do anything but make sure the family survives
they basically develop schizophrenia.  My reading on this is that Bateson
saw no way out of this, although it was one of the seminal articles in the
development of family therapy.  He brought it up again, but if I remember
correctly in the context of the intransience of poverty and/or
marginalization.  While individuals living within certain contexts may
understand the difficulties that these context present to them, they also
offer a way of bringing coherence to the world that makes their lives
livable.  So for instance people living in difficult areas understand the
difficulties and dangers that a street economy presents that is also the
type of activities they believe brings them a minimum level of
survivability.  You find this for instance with homeless youth.  Bateson
suggested you had to find a way of dealing with both issue simultaneously,
but for the most part seemed really frustrated.

 

So here are my interrelated questions.  Is the change laboratory basically
therapuetic in nature.  I know there is quite a bit of discussion of
interventions.  Relating this back to the target article then I am not sure
exactly what they are getting at.  The difference in the learning spheres I
don't think could be thought of as a double bind and I am not even sure it
is a contradiction.  It is instead it seems to me just a difference between
the two learning spheres, that you find in the United States quite a bit as
well.  I remember teaching high school a quarter of a century ago (oy) and
all the discussions about how there is no relationship between education
courses and the problems you face in the classroom.  If there was going to
be innovation from this one thinks it would have happened a while ago.  But
like I said, it doesn't drive me as a contradiciton driving towards an
innovation.  In any case what is the response to this difference.  How do
you actually deal with it beside talking about it.  This is a really big
thing in teacher education no doubt, but it is recognized and gets talked
about a lot.

 

It is interesting that these actually is a double bind in U.S. education
right now that is having terrible implications.  The emphasis on
standardized tests is forcing teachers to educate to the test.  At the same
time many especially experienced teachers see their main concern as keeping
students interested an in school within difficult circumstances.  One of the
worst things you can do is use a scorched earth policy, in which you get
higher test scores over the course of the year but lose the student over the
long term.  This is what the Teach for America teachers sometimes do I think
which is one of the reasons there is such animosity.  I talk to some
teachers and they tell me they either leave or go crazy.  Like Bateson I
can't see much a way out of this double bind.

 

Michael

 

  _____  

From: xmca-bounces@weber.ucsd.edu on behalf of smago
Sent: Wed 9/15/2010 6:20 AM
To: eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity
Subject: RE: [xmca] Where and What Is A Boundary?( is it betweenmicrogenesis
and ontogenesis?)

Thanks David. I would just like to clarify my own reservations about the use
of the triangle. As I see it, it works entirely well for Engeström's
organizational research, and he has had many iterations of it in different
organizational settings in order to develop and refine his categories and
depict them relationally through the triangle. And perhaps it works for
other sorts of organizational work, including that done for consulting fees,
which YE has assured us, I hope to our satisfaction, is not the approach he
takes.

There's a forthcoming article by YE and Annalisa Sannino, due in Educational
Research Review, that clarifies what his work is designed to do ("Studies of
expansive learning: Foundations, findings and future challenges) and traces
its evolution. It both clarified things for me and raised additional
questions. For instance, they outline "the theory of expansive learning" as
the framework for their research. But it seems that the theory of expansive
learning is taken to be synonymous with activity theory, or
cultural-historical activity theory. This conflation of terms and constructs
lies at the root of much of my confusion about how to refer to this work,
and perhaps others can help me understand whether they are viewed as
synonymous or whether there are distinctions that enable people not working
to change the object of collectives can be said to work as activity
theorists, or CHAT-ists.

I'll string together a series of quotes next to help clarify how Engeström
and Sannino explain their framework. Expansive learning is movement from
actions to activity. The essence of expansive learning activity is
production of objectively, societally new activity structures out of actions
manifesting the inner contradictions of the preceding form of activity in
question. Expansive learning is mastery of expansion from actions to a new
activity. Expansive learning typically calls for formative interventions
based on the principle of double stimulation. The theory of expansive
learning focuses on learning processes in which the very subject of learning
is transformed from isolated individuals to collectives and networks.
Expansive learning is manifested primarily as changes in the object of the
collective activity. Boundary crossing involves collective concept
formation. In formative interventions, the researcher aims at provoking and
sustaining an expansive transformation process led and owned by the
practitioners. A Change Laboratory is typically conducted in an activity
system that is facing a major transformation. To facilitate analysis and
resolution of the problems, interventionists typically introduce conceptual
tools such as the triangular models of activity systems as second stimulus.
Expansive learning takes place because historically evolving contradictions
in activity systems lead to disturbances, conflicts and double binds that
trigger new kinds of actions among the actors. Expansive learning is a
process of [collective] concept formation.

OK, that's sufficient for my purposes here. The article is much longer and
richer than I could reduce to a digest. I've made only one change in
Engeström and Sannino's language, which is the bracketed [collective] in the
final quote.

I am providing this summary in order to argue that most of what I see in the
invocation of the triangle is inappropriate, at least in educational
research in the US, which is primarily my concern. I see a role for it if a
group of educators understands a need for a change in the object of their
activity--say, a faculty responding to an initiative to move from learning
that is meaning-oriented to learning that produces high test scores (to give
a repugnant example)--and seeks a collective expansion to accommodate this
new disturbance in their work. But aside from what Viv Ellis is doing in the
U.K., I don't see it used that way, and given the US's emphasis on
individual rather than collective action, it's not an easy construct to
import. Rather, the triangle makes an appearance and then the researcher
does something that has nothing to do with changing the object of collective
activity.

My apologies for this long note, but I hope it helps to situate YE's work in
this discussion of the MCA article we have chosen to explore. p



-----Original Message-----
From: xmca-bounces@weber.ucsd.edu [mailto:xmca-bounces@weber.ucsd.edu] On
Behalf Of David Kellogg
Sent: Tuesday, September 14, 2010 8:07 PM
To: Culture ActivityeXtended Mind
Subject: Re: [xmca] Where and What Is A Boundary?( is it between
microgenesis and ontogenesis?)

eric--

I think that BOTH your example and Professor Smagorinsky's paper (for which
many thanks) really DO help to clarify the concept of boundaries.

I come to the word "boundary" from the work of people like Ben Rampton,
whose model is bilingualism, biracialism, bisexuality, and so on. Boundary
crossing here really is "interactional" in the sense that our authors would
like to see it: I become bilingual by crossing linguistic boundaries, from
one language to another and back again.

Professor Smagorinsky's paper notes that the "boundary" between theory and
practice is honored in the breach in precisely this way: by emphasizing the
need and the difficulty of bridging the gap we are indirectly acknowledging
the gap as a real phenomenon and prizing the ability of a few
preternaturally endowed persons amongst us to scuttle back and forth across
the divide.

There IS a real divide which we need help negotiating as teachers, and it's
a divide that teacher education alone can overcome. It's really not between
"theory" and "practice". It's  between the genetic laws that govern
ontogenesis, child development from year to year, and those that govern
microgenesis, changes in knowledge states from moment to moment.

These two things are not the same; the boundary between them is very real,
and it is (as we know) NOT addressed by Piaget and the constructivists (for
whom development "explains" learning) and ignored by most other approaches
(for whom developmetn just IS learning).

What eric suggests is that there is also a real boundary which belies the
unreal divorce between theory and practice which Smagorinsky, Cook and
Johnson seek to overcome by the concept (which unlike "knowledge" is a real
link between microgenesis and ontogenesis). Your example suggests a real
boundary between the NC and the lesson goals.

As eric says, this boundary is real, but it's an artefact of the (sometimes
genuinely destructive, occasionally helpful, but mostly irrelevant) attempt
of people outside the classroom to have their say about what goes on inside,
by means of guidelines, goals, achievement targets, etc. That's what I meant
by the underlying issue of class conflict.

Now, here's my question, which I think picks up some of Professor
Smagorinsky's criticisms of the way Engestrom's triangle is dragged in by
its long hairy ears at the drop of a business consulting hat. Why do we have
to classify these guidelines, goals, achievement targets etc. as "tools" and
not as, say, "rules" or "division of labor" or whatever?

I think the "expanded triangle" has both TOO MANY categories (because it
really separates out things like "tools" and "rules" and "division of labor"
that are not, in practice, any different from each other) and NOT ENOUGH
(because, for example, it does NOT distinguish between tools and signs, two
things that behave very differently in terms of their orientation to
objects). Worse, it does not differentiate between object-as-product and
object-as-process.

In what sense is a "learning trajectory" an "object of activity"? A training
programme does not produce "learning trajectories"--at the very most, it can
produce exam scores, diplomas, certificates and so on. "Learning
trajectories" are neither produced, exchanged, nor consumed in anything but
an extremely dull metaphor. Despite the way we may feel some days, learning
is not the consumption of teachers.

David Kellogg
Seoul National University of Education



On Tue, 9/14/10, ERIC.RAMBERG@spps.org <ERIC.RAMBERG@spps.org> wrote:


From: ERIC.RAMBERG@spps.org <ERIC.RAMBERG@spps.org>
Subject: Re: [xmca] Where and What Is A Boundary?( is it between scientific
and everyday concepts?)
To: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
Date: Tuesday, September 14, 2010, 9:22 AM


Being a psychology major I brought my ideas pertaining to learning
theories to the table when I began my studies for becoming a teacher.  I
was amazed that the majority of my classes had nothing to do with how to
provide environments where student experienced genuine learning but rather
focused on classroom management and providing 'checklist style' lesson
plans.

The current paper for discussion certainly addresses a core issue of
education.  I have been a 'student/teacher' as well as a mentor and have
discovered the difficulties faced when the 'scientific thinking' of
academia conflict with the 'everyday thinking' of being a teacher.  Where
the boundary is in this I am not quite clear other than the dept of
Education does the telling, the university does the disseminating of the
department's telling and the student/teacher is left to practice in the
reality of the classroom.

Teaching in an alternative program I have never had to provide syllabus'
for my classes because the instruction and the outcome are specifically
the same.  Is the student attending the science museum explorer's club and
do they have the project completed?  Is the student writing the resume,
filling out applications, scheduling job interviews and following through
with employment?  At what level of independence does the student operate?
Now a new regime shift has directed us to provide syllabus' for our
classes, the new administrator's don't want the syllabus' to appear in the
'everyday thinking' style of the questions above they want goals, outcomes
attached to MN state standards.  Is this possible to do?  Of course it is
possible but it has nothing to do with student success, if it did we
wouldn't have the 75% graduation rate (twice the national average for
special education students) we have experienced over the past 15 years.
Successful curriculum writing has nothing to do with genuine learning but
everything to do with complying to the political winds of the time.  How
this is a boundary I really truly don't know.

what do others think?
eric




From:   MICHAEL W SMITH <mwsmith@temple.edu>
To:     "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
Date:   09/14/2010 11:02 AM
Subject:        Re: [xmca] Where and What Is A Boundary?
Sent by:        xmca-bounces@weber.ucsd.edu



I share David's concern with understanding just what a boundary is.  I'd
like to add an additional question: How does one determine what boundaries
are relevant?  Put another way, how does the analytic help one identify
the
activity systems in operation and hence the boundaries that one needs to
examine.  The authors posit two systems:  the DTE and the cooperating
schools.  They posit the existence of these systems but don't appear to
derive them empirically.  That is, if the DTE and the schools are, in
fact,
the systems that are germane, one would be able to find sufficient
similarities in the "general object,"  in the rules, in the tools, and in
the division of labor across cooperating schools, small groups,
supervisors,
and so on.  As Peter pointed out in his initial post and as he has argued
in
his own research (Smagorinsky & O'Donnell-Allen, 1998, for example), it
might be a mistake to assume those similarities.  The key system, then,
might
be much smaller--an idioculture and not a culture.  In such a
circumstance,
investigating borders would not be investigating the spaces between the
DTE
and the schools, say, but would be investigating the spaces between the
small groups in Extract 2 and the DTE (or a particular university class)
or
investigating the spaces between the supervisor-student teacher-mentor
group
and both the particular cooperating school and the DTE in Excerpt 4.  My
worry about positing the larger structures first is that doing so might
keep
one from observing boundaries that are more important.


On Mon, Sep 13, 2010 at 9:40 PM, David Kellogg
<vaughndogblack@yahoo.com>wrote:

> First of all, many many thanks to Professor Peter Smagorinsky for his
> incisive but very fair summary of the study, and above all for opening
up
> issues that really go far beyond the study itself.
>
> As we see! With all due respect to Professor Engestrom and the folks at
the
> Change Lab, I think that the issue of profit, and salaries, and even
> critical peer review and publishing is much more interesting to the
writers
> of a study than it is to its readers. So I think that Professor
> Smagorinsky's understanding reflects, accurately, the reader's point of
> view.
>
> It certainly reflects mine. Studies like the one that Professor
Engestrom
> reports on in "Perspectives in Activity Theory" (CUP 1999: 377-404) as
> essentially examples of business consultancy discourse, in that they
focus
> on enhancing collaborative production, often through negotiated
> consensus, and leave the underlying issues of class conflict in the
> background. Nevertheless, it seems to me that precisely these issues are
the
> key to understanding the 'boundary" concept.
>
> I usually have a lot of trouble with articles like this. I am a deep
> breathing reader, and I can stay under water for a very long time
without a
> good definition. But my imagination (and understanding) is too visual
and
> concrete and I cannot really even begin to get a good picture of what
the
> author is going on about unless I have a good example.
>
> And I find I don't really get a good example of the "boundary" concept
> until p. 223, where we get a pretty clear boundary between what the
trainees
> have been told to do as trainees and what they will actually do as
teachers.
> This comes up again near the end, where there is a boundary set between
> "goals" (as defined by the NC) and "work", as defined by the teachers.
>
> I found myself wondering, as I read the study, if this boundary is not a
> temporary mirage created by the fact that we are talking about
PRE-SERVICE
> teacher education, a situation where the student teachers still have to
jump
> through hoops to get their certification, and where, because the student
> teachers do not have the experienced teachers' source of authority, viz,
> their chalkface experience, they are more or less at the mercy of
mentors,
> the professors, and of course the examiners.
>
> Pre-service programmes focus on what is teachable in preservice
programmes.
> That is why they focus so much on lesson plans, which in real life are
> notorious unreliable guides to classroom interaction for the simple
reason
> that they largely pre-empt it with "goals" and "targets" and "projected
> outcomes". They also focus on the kind of teaching knowledge that is
> statable and testable. But a great deal of this statable, plannable,
> rehearsable, and testable teaching knowledge is what Whitehead would
call
> "inert" knowledge.
>
> For example: one of the really key problems that teachers face when they
> start teaching in English is being able to reduce questions to the point
> where they are manageable and then expand them again to the point where
they
> are challenging to learners(who can be of widely varying degrees of
> proficiency, communicative capacity and even amiability). This was the
point
> of the distinction our teacher Ms. Yi Minkyeong made between "nonverbal
> response wanted", "verbal response wanted", and  "verbal thinking
wanted".
>
> This knowledge is not only largely unconscious, it is largely
> undescribable, because it includes (at least) three undescribed things:
> familiarity ("Have I said this before?"), structural complexity ("How
long
> and complex is the question? What about the answer?"), conceptual
complexity
> ("How concrete/visualizeable abstract/conceivable is the question? What
> about the answer?"). It's not the sort of thing we can teach in our
> pre-service programmes, although we are making some inroads with the
> in-service ones (where I am teaching this very afternoon).
>
> One of my grads is now studying the possibility of discarding the
> "artificial zone of proximal learning" of the pre-service programme
> altogether and going to the naturally occuring sort of "legitimate
> peripheral participation" that we find in apprenticeships, what we found
in
> for centuries in the arts and even in teaching before colleges of
education
> were invented. (I remember travelling on trucks in the Sudan and
watching
> how young Sudanese boys learned to become "drivers" in this way.)
>
> Shin Jiyeong has already found a significant difference in one novice
> teacher in the use of visual prompts and also in what she calls
> "non-repetition questions", that is, questions which produce the target
> sentences (specified by the NC) without actually asking for "Listen and
> Repeat". This is interesting because Shin Jieun and Kellogg 2007 failed
to
> find ANY improvement in another co-teacher in any part of the lesson
EXCEPT
> the greeting.
>
> What makes the difference? I think that Jinyeong's apprenticeship is NOT
a
> mentorship, where the relations found between examiners and examinees
> invariably reproduce themselves, and change the focus from the kind of
> indescribable knowledge we are looking for to the more describable sort.
> It's also NOT the crazy situation I started her out on (silly me) where
the
> two teachers had to teach the same thing at opposite ends of the
classroom
> looking at each other teach.
>
> First of all, the two teachers have good reasons to take turns watching
and
> teaching. One is an inexperienced native speaker of English and the
other is
> a highly expert non-native speaker, so they both have (different) things
to
> do and so different things to look at.
>
> Secondly, there is a clear cycle of "watch this" and "now you have a go"
> for both teachers. It seems to me where the actual teaching "work" is
not
> statable, this is essential: there is literally no other way for the
> knowledge to be internalized except through imitation, and the way our
short
> term memories work, there has to be a very short window between the
> observation and the imitation for the imitation to be accurate and
useful.
>
> Finally, there is, actually, an emergent concept, which neither teacher
was
> aware of at the beginning of Jiyeong's research, and it's not "goal"
whether
> of "work" or of "lesson" or of the NC. We started out just looking at
what
> kinds of behaviors produced long answers (more than one word) from the
kids,
> and we found to our distress that "Listen and Repeat" were virtually the
> only ones that did. I think the idea of a "non-imitative" or
"non-repetitive
> question" is still a bit of a broad brush. But I also think that when
the
> research is really done, this is where the real source of improvement
will
> come in, and I think, not coincidentally, that this is where pre-service
> programmes really have to butt out.
>
> I was reading the "Boundary" article, I set up a gedankenexperiment,
which
> I think reflects pretty well the kind of class conflict underlying the
> "boundary" concept. Let us say that there is a fundamental dispute over
a
> curricular issue. Imagine, for example, that a party comes to power that
> wishes to remove a fundamental concept from the curriculum (e.g. the
welfare
> state, or Norway's membership in the NATO alliance, participation in the
> invasion of Iraq, and active involvement in the Afghan War).
>
> Do I, as a teacher, want the decision making in the hands of a
CONSERVATIVE
> teacher (they do exist; I have quite a few as grad students) or in the
hands
> of a LIBERAL, even a RADICAL Department of Teacher Education?
>
> Well, OK, it's a thought experiment. The idea of a radical DTE is really
a
> little like one of Einstein's relativistic trains, or Schrodinger's cat;
> there are none such, and none likely. But I think that in actual fact
the
> real curricular decisions are ENTIRELY in the hands of the teacher, for
much
> the same reason that decisions over learning are in the final analysis
in
> the hands and heads of learners. And I think that's where they should be
> too.
>
> David Kellogg
> Seoul National University of Education
>
>
>
>
>
>
>
> _______________________________________________
> xmca mailing list
> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>



--
Michael W. Smith
Professor and Chair
Department of Curriculum, Instruction,
and Technology in Education
Temple University
1301 Cecil B.Moore Avenue
Philadelphia, PA 19122
_______________________________________________
xmca mailing list
xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca



-----Inline Attachment Follows-----


_______________________________________________
xmca mailing list
xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca




_______________________________________________
xmca mailing list
xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca

<<attachment: winmail.dat>>

_______________________________________________
xmca mailing list
xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca