[Date Prev][Date Next][Thread Prev][Thread Next][Date Index][Thread Index]

Re: [xmca] Mary Had a Little Lamb: the concept categories



Most of our elementary English book consists of what we might call "frozen pairs". This is the sort of thing you might learn to go on holiday in Italy.
 
A: Hello.
B: Hello.
 
A: I'm sorry.
B: That's okay.
 
A: Can you go skating with me?
B: Sounds good.
 
A: How are you?
B: I'm fine.
 
These frozen pairs are unifunctional. They are structurally monolithic. Genetically, they are learned as wholes and they are not analyzable (the child cannot say "You are fine", or "he is fine" or even "I am not fine". ). 
 
Karmiloff-Smith would say that they are at the IMPLICIT level of description; there is no conscious control of their function or their structure and there can be no development until they are redescribed. 
 
So I think that (structurally, functionally, and genetically, to use the three kinds of analysis that LSV employs over and over again) these "frozen pairs" correspond to syncretisms, in the Piagetian sense, and to "heaps" in the Vygotskyan one ("massa" in Meccaci's translation). They can be analyzed, modulated, manipulated and callibrated by others, but not by the child him or herself, and they are neither concepts nor even complexes.
 
Of course, all languages have these frozen pairs, and they are very useful for performing certain extremely circumscribed functions (especially at the beginnings and endings of real conversations and in quite well defined and highly repetitive transactional situations). 
 
But that is not why they are so popular. They are popular because:
 
a) there are a very small number of them and they can be taught in a flash
b) they are used by almost everybody and can be very widely marketed
c) they are self-contained and unproductive, so they can be readily tested (the pesky imagination of the learner doesn't get in the way).
 
Hucksters and cheats in language teaching are always trying to persuade us that that is all there is to language, but the kids know better. Here's a role play one of my grads just did. The monster created by Dr. Frankenstein has been disowned by his creator,and he has removed to the shores of Lake Geneva. 

T: There is a family. The family is a father, a daughter and a son. And the monster looks at the daughter. He wants to be friends with her. But he's worried. "Oh, what if the daughter runs away? I'm so ugly." And one day, he tries to talk to her. And she is really scared. Okay? We're going to play the scene. First, the monster says... everyone?
Ss: Hello, how are you?
T: The daughter says?
Ss: Hi, I'm fine. 
T: The monster says?
Ss: Can you go skating with me?
T: The daughter says?
Ss: Sorry, I can't skate. 
T: The monster says?
Ss: It's okay. I can teach you.
T: The girl says?
Ss: Sorry, but I have to do my homework. 
T: The monster says?
Ss: Is it because I'm ugly?
T: The girl says?
Ss: No, no, no, you are not ugly. 
T: But in his, no... in her head, she's thinking...?
Ss: Yes, yes, yes, you are so ugly.
 
The frozen pairs are still there, but they have been softened by the warmth of the chidlren's lips, and they have become at least partially analyzeable and controllable. The children can say "Sorry but..." and they can even apply negation ("No you are not ugly") and contrast verbal and mental processes ("Yes, you are so ugly"). 
 
These phrases are only partially creative (and if we look at them in transformational grammar terms we often find that they are separated from frozen pairs by only a single translation). They are also very much a response to the concrete, factual circumstances created by the teacher and by the previous utterance. 
 
And so I think that by and large they correspond to Piaget's concrete operations, and Vygotsky's complexes. Karmiloff Smith would say that they are at the E1 level of description, where knowledge is productive, but the actual rules for that productivity are not statable or even available to consciousness.
 
But that is not ALL we find in this role play. Look at this:
 
Ss: Is it because I'm ugly?
 
There is no structural or even functional precedent for this structure ANYWHERE in our textbook. Structurally, our book contains NO examples of hypotaxis (that is, subordination of one clause to another). Functionally, our book has NO incidents of a character attempting to elicit an unpleasant truth from another character who is attempting to shield the first character's feelings by denying the truth. 
 
Where, then, does it come from? I am tempted to say that it comes from Chapter Six of Thinking and Speech, where Vygotsky and Schif attempt to get children to create concepts like this by breaking off normal sentences after the use of "because" or "although".
 
Piaget would say that they are at the level of formal operations; they have a model of sentence structure that does not depend on content (and in this I think he would be wrong). Karmiloff-Smith would say that they have succeeded in making their performative knowledge explicit, although it is not yet declarative. 
 
But I think the true answer is probably far more wonderful. Structurally, the children have grasped the principle that one utterance can be embedded inside another ("Is it because...?") through what you call a process of particular generalization (through having heard the parts separately) and expanded abstraction (perhaps having heard similar structures before). Functionally, they have grasped the even more useful principle that people do not always say what they mean and we must sometimes do it for them.
 
David Kellogg
Seoul National University of Education
 

--- On Mon, 3/29/10, Steve Gabosch <stevegabosch@me.com> wrote:


From: Steve Gabosch <stevegabosch@me.com>
Subject: Re: [xmca] Mary Had a Little Lamb: the concept categories
To: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
Date: Monday, March 29, 2010, 12:46 PM


Just some brainstorming.  Here is a suggestion for a way to incorporate both potential concepts and pseudoconcepts from Ch 5 into the Ch 6 system (syncretic concepts, complexes, preconcepts, true concepts).

Perhaps the Ch 6 preconcept could be viewed as a combination of complexive thinking (concrete particular generalizing) and the potential concept (expanded abstraction).  'Preconceptual' thinking, of course, is not yet fully abstract and/or concrete general thinking (thinking using true or academic concepts).  Vygotsky's description of the relations of generality in terms of arithmetic seems suggestive of this way of looking at the preconcept.

And perhaps the Ch 5 pseudoconcept could be either a complex or a preconcept.  Perhaps both complexes and preconcepts could masquerade as true concepts, both in children and adults.

- Steve






On Mar 28, 2010, at 2:21 PM, David Kellogg wrote:

> Mike:
> 
> I too would like to hear what historians of the Kharkov school (and even Anton, who denies that such a separate school really exists) have to say about the decision to leave Moscow. There must have been VERY powerful reasons, not only because Moscow was, even more than in the time of Chekhov, the pulsing national centre of life, but because Kharkov was already on the verge of famine (the famin'e which Ukrainians call Holodomor, which is in so many ways similar to the Chinese famine of 1959-1961, the largest famine in recorded history).
> 
> Chukovsky, in his book "From Two to Five" reports meeting some Kharkovites in Odessa. He has very disparaging things to say about them, and this probably reflects official sentiment, Chukovsky was extremely hostile to Krupskaya, Blonsky, and pre1929 Soviet education (understandably, given the official response to his book "Crocodile"). There is an article on this (by me) in the current issue of Journal of Language and Literacy in Education:
> 
> http://www.coe.uga.edu/jolle/2010_1/Kellogg_reflecting.pdf
> 
> (The article is somewhat the worse for last minute editing changes, including to the title, which I did not see and didn't approve, but which I apologize for anyway.)
> 
> Steve:
> 
> Yes, it's quite possible to read the changes between Chapter Five and Chapter Six as a political capitulation on Vygotsky's part. That is exactly what Peter Langford does in his book "Vygotsky's Educational and Developmental Psychology" (Psychology Press, 2005),
> 
> But Langford considers the whole of Vygotsky's oeuvre to be little better than fraudulent, and in fact uses the word "academic fraud" several times in his book. He also thinks that Vygotsky was unacquainted with Marxism and that his sole source of theory was Hegel (rather the opposite of Andy's position). Finally, he insists in several places that Vygotsky was the inventor of something called "scaffolding" which he associates with outmoded and risibly romantic ideas he refers to as "progressive education"'. So Langford has a vested interest in showing how Vygotsky turned his back on these ideas, and there is every reason to take his argument with a block or even a mine of salt.
> 
> You are certainly right to say that the term "complex" was not coined by Vygotsky. Even the term "pseudoconcept" was borrowed from the Germans (Clara and Wilhelm Stern, in fact). Nor was the Suchmethod his invention; that he took from Rimat and Ach and it was already adapted by Uznadze by the time that Vygotsky and Sakharov used it.
> 
> Nevertheless, there is a LOT in Chapter Five that really is original with Vygotsky (see the attached Notes from our translation of 5.1) and Langford is right to trace the origins of this very striking originality to a direct reading of Hegel (in my view). I think Langford is, despite his extreme intellectual bias against Vygotsky, also correct in his assertion that there is a major shift in Vygotsky's thinking between 1929 and 1934, and that some of this can be associated with a shift away from complexes, at least by that name.
> 
> But as YOU pointed out, it is simply NOT true that Vygotsky completely gives up on Chapter Five in Chapter Six. He DOES refer to heaps, and to complexes, and also to concepts. There are only two obvious changes and they seem related:
> 
> a) Vygotsky drops the discussion of "potential concepts". But if you remember, the discussion of potential concepts is incoherent in Chapter Five; it's quite unclear if these are a completely separate stage, similar to heaps, complexes and concepts, or if they are somehow a sub-stage of concepts or something freestanding from both. No matter how you resolve this issue, the existence of "potential concepts" is an embarrassment to Vygotsky's clear assertion that the "pseudoconcept" is the last step before the creation of true concepts.
> 
> b) Vygotsky drops all mention of the "pseudoconcept" too, although he clearly does still believe in the Hegelian idea of a concept for others but not for myself. Instead of the "pseudoconcept" and the "potential concept" we have the "preconcept". But the preconcept is the main building block (if you will pardon the metaphor) throughout the elementary school period; in other words, it has exactly the role that Chapter Five assigns to complexive thinking in general and to pseudoconceptual thinking in particular. So I think the "preconcept" is none other than our old friend the "pseudoconcept".
> 
> And then there is the Sakharov mystery. Why, as ulvi's great poet Nazim Hikmet put it, did Benerdji kill himself? In 1929 the "Joint Opposition" between Zinoviev, Kamenev, and Krupskaya on the one hand and Trotsky, Preobrazhensky, and Rakovsky on the other disintegrated, because the Leningrad Opposition gave in to Bukharin and Stalin. The Chinese Revolution was definitively crushed, and the country took a sudden right turn, something James Cannon, visiting Moscow, became aware of in reading the Critique of the Draft Programme of the Communist International (Cannon says it was distributed by mistake, but it seems much more likely that it was a deliberate action by followers of Zinoviev).
> 
> Part of this hard right turn was the conservative critique of pedology which Langford still finds so very congenial to his way of thinking. You remember that many oppositionists, including Joffe, killed themselves. It seems to me quite possible that Sakharov might have been one of these.
> 
> David Kellogg
> Seoul National University of Education
> 
> 
> --- On Sun, 3/28/10, Steve Gabosch <stevegabosch@me.com> wrote:
> 
> 
> From: Steve Gabosch <stevegabosch@me.com>
> Subject: Re: [xmca] Mary Had a Little Lamb
> To: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
> Date: Sunday, March 28, 2010, 11:03 AM
> 
> 
> David, thank you so much, your work on this is very helpful indeed.  The footnotes by Seve and Meccaci you translated, as well as your commentary, open up all kinds of new questions for me that I really need to be asking.  Some things you bring up are very surprising for me.
> 
> This is for David, and anyone who might know: who has written on the history and politics of education in the USSR in Vygotsky's time?  I don't see how I can really understand his theories, especially related to concept formation and education, without understanding these things much better than I certainly do now.
> 
> The footnotes from Seve and Meccaci you translated - and Vygotsky's text on pg 211-212 in the Minick in Ch 6.4 - seem to be saying that the terms "complexes" and "complex system" and so forth were commonly used throughout the school system 1923-1931 ... and that these "complexes" that dominated pedagogical policy and practice are the **same** complexes that Vygotsky talks about Ch 5.
> 
> All this time I thought Vygotsky and his colleagues had **discovered** complexive thinking in the block experiment in 1928.
> 
> Apparently something quite different was the case.  Not only was complexive thinking not 'discovered' in 1928 by Sakharov and Vygotsky, but the entire Soviet school system was already based on deliberately **reinforcing** it!
> 
> Now, **that** is a twist I never saw coming.
> 
> Moreover - and this is also totally new to me, having apparently been completely oblivious of all this until now - the well-studied concepts of 'learning leads development' and the 'zone of proximal development' are conscious **reversals** of this 1923-1931 pedagogical policy called "complex systems".  Putting it somewhat provocatively, instead of schools reinforcing **complexes**, Vygotsky in 1933-1934 began to advocate that schools should now push **concepts**.
> 
> But was it all really that simple?  Vygotsky seems to make it seem so in his short description in 6.4.  But what kinds of political pressures was he under at the time to say certain things, and not say others?  I am not quite sure what to think about Ch 5 and Ch 6 now.
> 
> I need to learn more about this whole history and the impact it had on Vygotsky - and the impact Vygotsky was trying to have on this history.
> 
> Where can I go?
> 
> - Steve
> 
> 
> 
> 
> 
> On Mar 26, 2010, at 3:51 PM, David Kellogg wrote:
> 
>> Steve:
>> 
>> I'm changing the subject line because it seems to me this is a specialist problem of particular interest to old reds with long memories. I DO think there is a key programmatic problem here of interest to ALL educators: whether or not the lamb of complexes should be left at the school door. But the historical problem we deal with here is, unfortunately, how the lamb got made into lamb chops.
>> 
>> I know you are a great stickler for refs, chapter and verse, so I begin by telling you where the refs are. The part you mention is dealt with on pp. 229-230 of the Chapter Six All document I sent you. Here's my understanding without the refs, all of which you will find either in that document or the attached, pp. 14-16.
>> 
>> Vygotsky and Sakharov were working with Krupskaya in Narkompros, and Vygotsky himself was one of a half dozen leaders of pedology, the dominant current in the Narkompros during the twenties.
>> 
>> But Krupskaya was a leader of the Leningrad Opposition to Stalin. This wasn't the Left Opposition led by Trotsky, although for a while it was in a bloc with the Trotskyists known as the Joint Opposition. It was something much more ideologically diffuse; a group of old Bolsheviks like Krupskaya herself, and of course the inseparable Zinoviev and Kamenev.
>> 
>> By1929, Stalin had completely undone the Leningrad Opposition by forming a bloc with the right wing of the party led by Bukharin. This group was socially very powerful and not exactly friendly to revolutionary turmoil in education; they rested on a rising tide of people tired of revolution and war and very pleased with the successes of the New Economic Policy.
>> 
>> The 1931 decree on pedology was, as many polemical decisions are, very sweeping: it condemned the WHOLE of Bolshevik educational policy from 1923 onwards. The Labor School, the system of teaching "complexes" of vocabulary associated with specific activities, and pedology as a separate, developmental discipline concerned with the whole child were all be to swept away as infantile disorders.
>> 
>> Sakharov killed hiimself. Krupskaya almost disappeared. But Vygotsky himself survived, and in some ways he even turned this catastrophe into a theoretical strength! In fact, as I argue in the attached, it was this maelstrom that formed the crucible for the zone of proximal development.
>> 
>> David Kellogg
>> Seoul National University of Education
>> 
>> --- On Fri, 3/26/10, Steve Gabosch <stevegabosch@me.com> wrote:
>> 
>> 
>> From: Steve Gabosch <stevegabosch@me.com>
>> Subject: Re: [xmca] The strange situation
>> To: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
>> Date: Friday, March 26, 2010, 6:54 AM
>> 
>> 
>> David, thank you for your explanations - very, very helpful.  You have been such a help for me on Ch 5 and  6 for some time now.
>> 
>> On your last point, your story about political pressures on Vygotsky to "leave complexes at the school door" -  where can I find out more about that?
>> 
>> I don't know if this is related - but I notice some paragraphs in the Minick pg 211-212 in Ch 6 section 4 that talk about "It is well known that when the complex system dominated school instruction it was said it was based on "pedagogical foundations."  It goes on to be very critical of this system of instruction.  What is Vygotsky talking about?
>> 
>> - Steve
>> 
>> 
>> On Mar 25, 2010, at 2:30 PM, David Kellogg wrote:
>> 
>>> Mike: Yes, the paper that Martin circulated from Rosch has what I think is a pretty good summary of the work you refer to and it's certainly relevant.
>>> 
>>> Both Davydov and Engestrom are what I would call "activity" interpretations of Chapter Six: they tend to de-emphasize the distinctness of SEMIOSIS from PERCEPTION. Martin calls this approach trying to squeeze concepts out of percepts.
>>> 
>>> Steve: Actually, this is just the returns on a short term loan, you know, YOU were the one who first showed me that preconcepts couldn't possibly be a generic name for all of the thinking that happens before concepts, and one of the quotes you gave to show this is in that list (although not in the form you gave, because once you have tasted Thinking and Speech in the non-English translations it's hard to go back to English rotgut).
>>> 
>>> Andy has a good therapist's approach to discussion; he tends to "hold up a mirror" to the patient by reformulating and recasting his own words. Our elementary school teachers do this very well too, but they tend to focus on grammar mistakes (of which there are quite a few in my original post!) I know, it's not really from therapy or from primary education; it's his training in immanent critique.
>>> 
>>> In any case, he's got me right. You notice that LSV does not use the word "pseudoconcept" anywhere in Chapter Six, although he does talk about syncretisms and complexes a fair amount. He also places the preconcept on the border between complexes and concepts. Finally, he says the preconcept is a concept that is generalized by others but not by the self. So I think the preconcept is the pseudoconcept, even though in Chapter Five he argued that psychologically speaking the pseudoconcept is a complex.
>>> 
>>> Why the change in nomenclature? Well, first of all, LSV often picks up a word from somebody else. Knocks it around a bit. Hollows it out and stuffs it with something else. And finally renames it. For example, the word "everyday concept" is clearly related to the "spontaneous concept" of Piaget (which Vygotsky actually refers to using Piaget's name in Chapter Two) but it's also quite different, because LSV totally rejects the elaborate methodology that Piaget uses to purge the child's spontaneous concepts of adult taint in "The Child's Conception of the World".
>>> 
>>> That's why I prefer Prout's translation of "academic concept" for the nonspontaneous concept (and even "self-directed speech" for private/egocentric speech, although I admit that Vygotsky never actually uses this term). So I think that arithmetic numbers are the preacademic form of this concept, but they are only one possible instantiation of the true, academic form, which is entirely relational and algebraic, which is why it forms the extreme North Pole (or South Pole, if you use Ana's map) of the measure of generality.
>>> 
>>> I also think that by translating the "science concept" as the academic concept we can include a range of higher concepts from ethics teaching and even aesthetics. Right now one of my students is using Vygotsky/Piaget's sentence completion method for testing pairs of everyday/academic concepts like "gift"/"loan", "road"/"avenue", and even 'so"/"because" and "but"/"although".
>>> 
>>> But secondly Vygotsky is trimming. All educators have been ordered to "leave complexes at the school door".  In 1931 he is still reeling from the declaration of the Bolshevik Party Central Committee on pedology; he has to abandon everything he's written about complexes in education because it is now considered that they do not belong in primary education at all. But he KNOWS that primary school children think in complexes, and not concepts.
>>> 
>>> Mary had a little lamb
>>> Whose fleece was white as snow
>>> And everywhere that Mary went
>>> The lamb was sure to go
>>> 
>>> So in 1934 he changes his tactics.  By rechristening the pseudoconcept as the "preconcept", by chanting the slogan of leaving the complex at the school door, and above all by introducing the concept of the zone of proximal development as part of a feigned critique of pedology, he is able to make sure that complexes are "left at the school door" in name only.
>>> 
>>> It followed her to school one day.
>>> Which was against the rules
>>> It made the children laugh and play
>>> To see a lamb at school...
>>> 
>>> David Kellogg
>>> Seoul National University of Education
>>> 
>>> --- On Thu, 3/25/10, mike cole <lchcmike@gmail.com> wrote:
>>> 
>>> 
>>> From: mike cole <lchcmike@gmail.com>
>>> Subject: Re: [xmca] The strange situation
>>> To: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
>>> Date: Thursday, March 25, 2010, 8:49 AM
>>> 
>>> 
>>> I will not err by entering into this discussion until I am sure I am not
>>> being irrelevant. Two brief comments that I think are probably not
>>> irrelevant.
>>> 
>>> 1. A lot of work has been done along lines initiated by eleanor r in the
>>> 1970's. I will check for an update of the current thinking along lines she
>>> started and see what I can find.
>>> 
>>> 2. It may be worth people's while to remember that Davydov had his own
>>> criticism of Vygotsky's notion of scientific concept which clearly does go
>>> back to Hegel. For a summary of the discussion, see Engestrom, Ch. 4  at
>>> 
>>> http://lchc.ucsd.edu/mca/Paper/Engestrom/expanding/ch4.htm
>>> 
>>> mike
>>> 
>>> On Thu, Mar 25, 2010 at 8:08 AM, Steve Gabosch <stevegabosch@me.com> wrote:
>>> 
>>>> Thank you VERY much for this, David.  You have just completely re-oriented
>>>> me to Ch6sect6 - I feel like I woke up and turned a light on and discovered
>>>> I had only been getting 1/3 of it, and now I am getting 2/3 of that
>>>> difficult and fascinating section.  This was extremely helpful.  I am
>>>> finding that the more I set aside what I thought I knew about concept
>>>> formation from Ch5, the more I understand Ch6.
>>>> 
>>>> What is your take on the relationship between the pseudoconcept of Ch5 and
>>>> the preconcept of Ch6?
>>>> 
>>>> - Steve
>>>> 
>>>> 
>>>> 
>>>> 
>>>> 
>>>> On Mar 24, 2010, at 10:02 PM, David Kellogg wrote:
>>>> 
>>>>     Andy, Steve:
>>>>> 
>>>>> Take a look at these. The translations are my own but the page numbers
>>>>> suggest the corresponding bits in your Minick translation.
>>>>> 
>>>>> “We have first of all succeeded in discovering that generality
>>>>> (differences in generality) does not coincide with the structure of
>>>>> generalization and its different stages such as we found them in our
>>>>> experimental study on the formation of concepts: syncretic images,
>>>>> complexes, preconcepts, and concepts. (roughly, p. 225 in your Minick)”
>>>>> 
>>>>> You can see from this that "preconcepts" is NOT a general term including
>>>>> syncretisms, complexes any more than "rose" is a general term including
>>>>> daisies and daffodils.
>>>>> 
>>>>> “In the first place, concepts of different generality are possible in a
>>>>> same generalization structure. For example, in the structure of concepts by
>>>>> complexes it is possible for concepts of different levels of generality to
>>>>> exist: flower, and rose. In truth, we must state a reservation from the very
>>>>> outset, that is to say that the relationship of generalization ”flower-rose”
>>>>> will be different in each structure of generalization, for example,
>>>>> different in the structure of complexes from in the structure of
>>>>> preconcepts.” (225)
>>>>> 
>>>>> We can see from this that LSV does NOT consider a preconcept to be a
>>>>> complex.
>>>>> 
>>>>> “Thanks to the analysis of the real concepts of the child, we have been
>>>>> able to study some less well-known properties of syncretic formations,
>>>>> complexes, and preconcepts and to establish what in each of these spheres of
>>>>> thinking is shown to be different in the relationship with the object as
>>>>> well as the apprehension of the object by thought, that is to say, how the
>>>>> two fundamental elements which characterize concepts are revealed to be
>>>>> different from one stage to another.” (228)
>>>>> 
>>>>> Once again, "preconcepts" are not the preconceptual functional equivalents
>>>>> of concepts (that is, they are not a hypernym for syncretic heaps and
>>>>> complexes). But here Vygotsky suggests that there are two processes and not
>>>>> one at work in concept formation.
>>>>> 
>>>>> One is indeed a form of activity: it's a relationship with the object,
>>>>> e.g. ostension, indication, and naming. But the other is "the apprehension
>>>>> of the object by thought", the way in which the object is represented
>>>>> (reflected/refracted/semiotically reproduced) by the mind.
>>>>> 
>>>>>     “What we have managed to establish here with respect to the passage from
>>>>> the preconcepts of the schoolchild to the concepts of the adolescent is the
>>>>> same thing that we managed to establish in the preceding study with respect
>>>>> to the passage of generalized perceptions to general representations, that
>>>>> is to say syncretic formations and complexes.” (230)
>>>>> 
>>>>> This appears to be a direct reference to Chapter Five. In 1931, LSV
>>>>> considered this to be a study of concept formation in ADOLESCENTS. But now
>>>>> he appears to have changed his mind: the previous chapter is concerned with
>>>>> the passage from generalized perceptions to general representations, and is
>>>>> thus a matter of preschoolers. This is quite consistent with what Paula did
>>>>> with three year olds to eight year olds.
>>>>> 
>>>>> “Just as in that case it turned out that a new stage in the development of
>>>>> generalizations can only be attained by the transformation, not the
>>>>> annulment, of the preceding stage, by the generalization of the objects
>>>>> already generalized, not by proceeding anew from the generalization of
>>>>> single objects, in the same way here the study has shown that the transition
>>>>> from preconcepts (of which the typical example is the arithmetical concept
>>>>> of the school child) to the true concepts of the adolescent (of which the
>>>>> typical example is the algebraic concept) happens through the generalization
>>>>> of objects which have already been generalized.”
>>>>> 
>>>>> And here we see why! The generalized perception is the PRECONDITION of the
>>>>> general representation. And the general representation is the precondition
>>>>> of the concept. The example he gives us is numbers.
>>>>> 
>>>>> Of course, at the very lowest level, numbers really are the result of the
>>>>> activity of the perceptible and perceptual activity of counting. But take
>>>>> away the objects, and the number remains as a generalized representation.
>>>>> And when we take away the number, and deal only with the realtion of number,
>>>>> the concept remains.
>>>>> 
>>>>> "The preconcept is the abstraction of the number, detached from the object
>>>>> and, founded on this abstraction, the generalization of the numerical
>>>>> properties of the object. The concept is the abstraction detached from the
>>>>> number and, founded on it, the generalization of any relation between
>>>>> numbers. But the abstraction and generalization of ideas differs
>>>>> fundamentally from the abstraction and the generalization of things. It is
>>>>> not a pursuit of movement in the same direction or its culmination, it is
>>>>> the beginning of a movement in a new direction, a transition to a new and
>>>>> higher plane of thinking. (230)"
>>>>> 
>>>>> This of course returns us to a point that Vygotsky made in the very first
>>>>> chapter and returns again to in the very last: the "dialectical leap" is not
>>>>> simply from inanimate to animate, but from perception to thinking.
>>>>> 
>>>>> There is a qualitative difference between the abstraction and
>>>>> generalization of perceptions and the abstraction and generalization of
>>>>> thoughts; they are distinct processes, and the word "activity" applies much
>>>>> more accurately to the former than the latter.
>>>>> 
>>>>> David Kellogg
>>>>> Seoul National University of Education
>>>>> 
>>>>> 
>>>>> 
>>>>> 
>>>>> --- On Wed, 3/24/10, Andy Blunden <ablunden@mira.net> wrote:
>>>>> 
>>>>> 
>>>>> From: Andy Blunden <ablunden@mira.net>
>>>>> Subject: Re: [xmca] The strange situation
>>>>> To: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
>>>>> Date: Wednesday, March 24, 2010, 9:30 PM
>>>>> 
>>>>> 
>>>>> Steve, briefly and without references, my take was:
>>>>> 
>>>>> * *preconcepts* are a family name for all the thought-forms prior to true
>>>>> concepts and so includes potential concepts, pseduoconcepts, complexes. etc.
>>>>> 
>>>>> * *potential concepts* are, as far as I can, see the highest type of
>>>>> pseudoconcept, marked by its "transferability" to different sensory fields.
>>>>> Here the attributes have been completely isolated from their substratum.
>>>>> 
>>>>> * *complex* is a family name for a whole group of forms including both
>>>>> pseudo- and potential concepts.
>>>>> 
>>>>> Andy
>>>>> 
>>>>> Steve Gabosch wrote:
>>>>> 
>>>>>> David, thanks again for these extremely useful files of your translations
>>>>>> of T&S from Meccaci, Seve, Prout, and your Korean team.  I am in awe of the
>>>>>> work you did, and are still doing.
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> I thought where we got stuck last year was on that pesky creature from Ch
>>>>>> 5, the 'potential concept,' not the clearer concept, 'pseudoconcept'.  I
>>>>>> think Vygotsky leaves no doubt that the pseudoconcept is a complex.  I am
>>>>>> still struggling with precisely what a potential concept is.
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> Both complicated concepts, potential concept and pseudoconcept, seem to
>>>>>> be subsumed into the Ch 6 term 'preconcept'.  That move gives us a simpler
>>>>>> term, but leaves many questions unanswered.  It leaves us little choice but
>>>>>> to investigate concept formation ourselves.
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> Martin, I would be most interested, when you have the time, if you took
>>>>>> your recent very excellent questions and reframed them, or more precisely,
>>>>>> sharpened them, in light of Ch 6.  I think some important work can be done
>>>>>> analyzing Ch 5 in terms of Ch 6 - and looking at Ch 6, especially section 6,
>>>>>> in terms of Ch 5.
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> Apparently about 3, 4 or 5 years did separate the main writing of these
>>>>>> two chapters, as you and Paula suggest.  On one hand, there is an explosion
>>>>>> of ideas in Ch 6 sect 6 that are barely touched on or anticipated in Ch 5.
>>>>>>     On the other hand, the rich, specific ideas in Ch 5 are insufficiently
>>>>>> dealt in light of the new, general ideas in Ch 6 sect 6.  Vygotsky left that
>>>>>> challenge to us as well.
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> - Steve
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> On Mar 24, 2010, at 5:35 PM, David Kellogg wrote:
>>>>>> 
>>>>>>     Martin, Steve:
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> Last night I showed a picture of an iguana to my graduate seminar and
>>>>>>> asked what it was. Everybody said it was an ALLIGATOR. This is strange,
>>>>>>> because the word "iguana" exists as a loan word from English in Korean, and
>>>>>>> in fact everybody confirmed that they knew the word, but the word
>>>>>>> "alligator" does not exist in Korean and instead we use a Chinese loan word
>>>>>>> (literally, "evil fish").
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> What this means is that my grads have the WORD but not the CONCEPT of
>>>>>>> Iguana--it is an example of a concept for others but not for myself. This is
>>>>>>> not the only situation where that is true, of course. For example, the words
>>>>>>> "Miss" and "Mister" also exist in Korean as loan words, but they are quite
>>>>>>> impolite and used to refer to social inferiors (bar girls, prostitutes,
>>>>>>> secretaries or waiters or male underlings of one kind or another). Here too
>>>>>>> the concept of the English polite form of address exists as a word but not
>>>>>>> as a concept.
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> Last year I suggested to Steve that in Chapter Six Vygotsky uses the
>>>>>>> word "preconcept" to refer to this situation, and that therefore the word
>>>>>>> "preconcept" is used in preference to "pseudoconcept" in Chapter Six. Steve
>>>>>>> objected that Chapter Five clearly says that a pseudoconcept is not a
>>>>>>> concept at all, but a complex, while Chapter Six says that it is indeed a
>>>>>>> concept, although not a concept for myself.
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> I'm still unconvinced. As Steve says there really IS a shift of opinion
>>>>>>> on a number of issues in Chapter Six (the carry over from one structure of
>>>>>>> generalization to another, for example, and also the issue of whether
>>>>>>> concepts can be taught to pre-adolescents). The word "pseudoconcept", which
>>>>>>> is so misleading that it even confuses LSV himself sometimes, is not LSV's
>>>>>>> coinage; he took it from the Sterns,who took it from somebody else.
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> So it seems to me that "pseudoconcept" in Chapter Five is a concept for
>>>>>>> others (for the Sterns), and it only becomes a concept for LSV himself in
>>>>>>> Chapter Six!
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>     David Kellogg
>>>>>>> Seoul National University of Education
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> Attached is OUR re-reading of Chapter Six, here in Seoul.Sorry about the
>>>>>>> Korean!
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> --- On Wed, 3/24/10, Martin Packer <packer@duq.edu> wrote:
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> From: Martin Packer <packer@duq.edu>
>>>>>>> Subject: Re: [xmca] The strange situation
>>>>>>> To: "eXtended Mind, Culture, Activity" <xmca@weber.ucsd.edu>
>>>>>>> Date: Wednesday, March 24, 2010, 12:57 PM
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> Thanks, Steve,
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> I've been putting off re-reading chapter 6, but I have to bite the
>>>>>>> bullet soon. I was thinking that trying to figure out what LSV had come up
>>>>>>> with and written about in chap 5 (and Paula has pointed out that he seems to
>>>>>>> have had this figured out by 1930) would itself be valuable. But you make a
>>>>>>> cogent argument.
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> Martin
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> On Mar 24, 2010, at 1:39 PM, Steve Gabosch wrote:
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>     These are really, really good questions, Martin.  All worth very
>>>>>>>> serious exploration.
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>> My take on Chapter 5, after doing some study of it, and Chapter 6, last
>>>>>>>> year with David Ke. and Paula T., and some discussion here on xmca, is that
>>>>>>>> Ch 5 might be best understood in terms of Chapter 6, especially section 6
>>>>>>>> starting on pg 224 of Vol 1.  Here Vygotsky gets to his major theoretical
>>>>>>>> discussion of systems of concepts, and critiques the limitations of the
>>>>>>>> block experiments on page 228 and 229.
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>> He explains that the block experiment "ignored the fact that **each new
>>>>>>>> stage in the development of generalization depends on the generalizations
>>>>>>>> found in the preceding stages.**"  pg 229.  He was critical of the block
>>>>>>>> experiment not revealing connections or transitions between the stages of
>>>>>>>> concept development.  He felt he was able to reveal these connections with
>>>>>>>> the experiments described in Chapter 6.
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>> It is important to emphasize that he does not at all **reject** the
>>>>>>>> work described in Chapter 5 - the syncretic heap, complexes, and what he now
>>>>>>>> calls preconcepts (was pseudoconcepts), and true concepts, are still intact
>>>>>>>> - but he **adds** a whole new level of theorizing that he saw as crucial -
>>>>>>>> suggestions for solutions to "the central problem" of his research in
>>>>>>>> Chapter 6, involving systems and relationships of generality, the law of
>>>>>>>> concept equivalence (any concept can be represented through other concepts
>>>>>>>> in an infinite number of ways), measures of generality, systems of concepts,
>>>>>>>> etc.  Vygotsky's most advanced thinking about concept formation is here in
>>>>>>>> this section.  And some of your very good questions are addressed.
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>> This is why I think that Chapter 5 needs to be seen as something of a
>>>>>>>> building block toward section 6 in Chapter 6, and that it might be easier to
>>>>>>>> read Chapter 6 sect 6 first and work backwards, or work them together as one
>>>>>>>> study.  And don't forget that Vygotsky's publisher or maybe even Vygotsky
>>>>>>>> himself got longitude and latitude backwards in the globe metaphor when he
>>>>>>>> explains the law of concept equivalence! (pg 226)  LOL
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>> Chapter 6 as a whole, of course, has much material on everyday vs
>>>>>>>> scientific concepts, as well as the oft-quoted passages on the zone of
>>>>>>>> proximal development, so that difficult section 6 in Ch 6 kind of gets
>>>>>>>> overshadowed, and maybe a little disconnected from Chapter 5.  The two need
>>>>>>>> to be dialectically joined, I believe, to really grasp what Vygotsky was
>>>>>>>> trying to do in both chapters.  And there is also some discussion on pg 189
>>>>>>>> in section 2 in Chapter 6, and maybe a few other places in that chapter,
>>>>>>>> about complexes and so forth, that may also shed some helpful light on some
>>>>>>>> specifics in Chapter 5.
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>> - Steve
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>> On Mar 23, 2010, at 1:25 PM, Martin Packer wrote:
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>     I am taking the liberty of recycling this subject heading, after
>>>>>>>>> having spent some time re-reading the posts over the weekend. I seem to have
>>>>>>>>> played a large part in hijacking this thread some time last year, with my
>>>>>>>>> obsession over the meaning of the term 'reflection.'
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>> So this message is partly penance, but it also me trying to make sense
>>>>>>>>> of LSV's block task and what it tells us about his views of concepts, and
>>>>>>>>> their development. I find myself with the following questions:
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>> 1. It seems to be the case that in chapter 5 LSV doesn't mention the
>>>>>>>>> distinction between everyday concepts and scientific concepts. Is it at all
>>>>>>>>> possible that what in chapter 6 he calls "everyday concepts" are what he
>>>>>>>>> refers to in chapter 5 as complexes? I suspect not, but the question seems
>>>>>>>>> worth asking.
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>> 2. LSV seems to offer not one but two explanations of how the child
>>>>>>>>> (or rather the adolescent) forms concepts. The first explanation is that
>>>>>>>>> concepts arise from the advanced application of the processes of
>>>>>>>>> generalization and abstraction, specifically that the word is now used
>>>>>>>>> functionally for voluntary control of attention, permitting a mastery of
>>>>>>>>> these processes. The second explanation is based on the phenotypical
>>>>>>>>> identity and functional similarity of concepts and pseudoconcepts. The
>>>>>>>>> latter are actually complexes, but they look like concepts and so when child
>>>>>>>>> and adult interact the adult takes them to be concepts. The child is in a
>>>>>>>>> sense then using concepts without knowing it, and LSV appeals to the
>>>>>>>>> familiar Hegelian process of in-itself, for-others, for-self, to explain how
>>>>>>>>> this "internal contradiction"is the "basic genetic prerequisite" for the
>>>>>>>>> rise of true concepts.
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>> I'm not suggesting that these two explanations are incompatible or
>>>>>>>>> mutually exclusive. But LSV does not seem to try to bring them together.
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>> 3. In other words, this second explanation is another case of
>>>>>>>>> "internalization," and the application of the general genetic law of
>>>>>>>>> cultural development. But LSV adds that this "peculiar genetic situation" in
>>>>>>>>> the move from pseudoconcepts to concepts should be considered the general
>>>>>>>>> rule rather than the exception in children's intellectual development. Does
>>>>>>>>> this not suggest that this same kind of process occurs as the child moves
>>>>>>>>> from heaps to complexes?
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>> 4. Generalization and abstraction are the two "channels" in the
>>>>>>>>> development of concepts - LSV refers to them also as "complexing" and
>>>>>>>>> "segregating." The first is very familiar by the time we get to chapter 5:
>>>>>>>>> he has been writing about the way a word is a generalization since the start
>>>>>>>>> (this is where as David has pointed out we find the quotation from Sapir.)
>>>>>>>>> But abstraction seems to appear out of nowhere. Is there a treatment of
>>>>>>>>> abstraction/segregating elsewhere in the book that I have missed?
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>> 5. LSV seems to get to the end of chapter 5 without ever telling us
>>>>>>>>> exactly what a concept it.  He suggests that it involves hierarchy, and
>>>>>>>>> connections that are abstract, essential, and homogeneous. He proposes that
>>>>>>>>> particular and general are linked. He adds that "most important" is "the
>>>>>>>>> unity of form and content," for this is what makes thinking in concepts a
>>>>>>>>> "real revolution." Can anyone pull these somewhat diverse (complexive?)
>>>>>>>>> characteristics together for me? Do they harmonize with the treatment of
>>>>>>>>> concepts (of both kinds) in chapter 6?
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>> 6. Finally, less a question than an observation. LSV writes at the
>>>>>>>>> close of chapter 5 of the way that “Concept thinking is a new form of
>>>>>>>>> intellectual activity, a new mode of conduct, a new intellectual mechanism.
>>>>>>>>> The intellect is able to find a new and unprecedented modus operandi in this
>>>>>>>>> particular activity and a new function becomes available within the system
>>>>>>>>> of intellectual functions which is distinctive both in its composition and
>>>>>>>>> structure as well as in the way it functions.” I take this as a clear
>>>>>>>>> indication that for LSV a concept is not simply a new kind of mental
>>>>>>>>> representation. It is, as Rosch proposes, a new way of relating to the
>>>>>>>>> world.
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>> Any guidance through this thicket will be gratefully accepted!
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>> Martin
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>>>>>>> xmca mailing list
>>>>>>>>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>>>>>>>>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>>>>>> xmca mailing list
>>>>>>>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>>>>>>>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>>>>> xmca mailing list
>>>>>>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>>>>>>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>>> <Chapter six all.doc>_______________________________________________
>>>>>>> xmca mailing list
>>>>>>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>>>>>>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>>>>>>> 
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>>>> xmca mailing list
>>>>>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>>>>>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>>>>>> 
>>>>>> 
>>>>> --
>>>>> ------------------------------------------------------------------------
>>>>> Andy Blunden http://www.erythrospress.com/
>>>>> Classics in Activity Theory: Hegel, Leontyev, Meshcheryakov, Ilyenkov $20
>>>>> ea
>>>>> 
>>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>>> xmca mailing list
>>>>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>>>>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>>>>> 
>>>>> 
>>>>> 
>>>>> 
>>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>>> xmca mailing list
>>>>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>>>>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>>>>> 
>>>> 
>>>> _______________________________________________
>>>> xmca mailing list
>>>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>>>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>>>> 
>>> _______________________________________________
>>> xmca mailing list
>>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>>> 
>>> 
>>> 
>>> 
>>> _______________________________________________
>>> xmca mailing list
>>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>> 
>> _______________________________________________
>> xmca mailing list
>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
>> 
>> 
>> 
>> <4Korean in English.doc>_______________________________________________
>> xmca mailing list
>> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
>> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
> 
> _______________________________________________
> xmca mailing list
> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
> 
> 
> 
> <Notes.doc>_______________________________________________
> xmca mailing list
> xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
> http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca

_______________________________________________
xmca mailing list
xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca




_______________________________________________
xmca mailing list
xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca