[xmca] When Language Can Hold the Answer (full text)

From: Michael Boatright <michaelboatright who-is-at gmail.com>
Date: Wed Apr 23 2008 - 14:13:08 PDT

[image: The New York Times]
<http://www.nytimes.com/><http://www.nytimes.com/adx/bin/adx_click.html?type=goto&page=www.nytimes.com/printer-friendly&pos=Position1&sn2=336c557e/4f3dd5d2&sn1=81bbf79c/2c0591c8&camp=foxsearch2008_emailtools_810902d-nyt5&ad=youngheart_88x31_8.gif&goto=http%3A%2F%2Fwww%2Efoxsearchlight%2Ecom%2Fyoungatheart>

------------------------------
April 22, 2008
When Language Can Hold the AnswerBy CHRISTINE KENNEALLY

Faced with pictures of odd clay creatures sporting prominent heads and
pointy limbs, students at Carnegie Mellon were asked to identify which
"aliens" were friendly and which were not.

The students were not told that the aliens fell naturally into two groups,
although the differences were subtle and not easy to describe.

Some had somewhat lumpy, misshapen heads. Others had smoother domes. After
students assigned each alien to a category, they were told whether they had
guessed right or wrong, learning as they went that smooth heads were
friendly and lumpy heads were not.

The experimenter, Dr. Gary Lupyan, who is now doing postdoctoral research
at Cornell<http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/organizations/c/cornell_university/index.html?inline=nyt-org>,
added a little item of information to one test group. He told the group that
previous subjects had found it helpful to label the aliens, calling the
friendly ones "leebish" and the unfriendly ones "grecious," or vice versa.

When the participants found out whether their choice was right or wrong,
they were also shown the appropriate label. All the participants eventually
learned the difference between the aliens, but the group using labels
learned much faster. Naming, Dr. Lupyan concluded, helps to create mental
categories.

The finding may not seem surprising, but it is fodder for one side in a
traditional debate about language and perception, including the thinking
that creates and names groups.

In stark form, the debate was: Does language shape what we perceive, a
position associated with the late Benjamin Lee Whorf, or are our perceptions
pure sensory impressions, immune to the arbitrary ways that language carves
up the world?

The latest research changes the framework, perhaps the language of the
debate, suggesting that language clearly affects some thinking as a special
device added to an ancient mental skill set. Just as adding features to a
cellphone or camera can backfire, language is not always helpful. For the
most part, it enhances thinking. But it can trip us up, too.

The traditional subject of the tug of war over language and perception is
color. Because languages divide the spectrum differently, researchers have
asked whether language affected how people see color. English, for example,
distinguishes blue from green. Most other languages do not make that
distinction. Is it possible that only English speakers really see those
colors as different?

Past investigations have had mixed results. Some experiments suggested that
color terms influenced people in the moment of perception. Others suggested
that the language effect kicked in only after some basic perception
occurred.

The consensus was that different ways to label color probably did not affect
the perception of color in any systematic way.

Last year, Lera Boroditsky and colleagues published a study in The Proceedings
of the National Academy of
Sciences<http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/organizations/p/proceedings_of_the_national_academy_of_sciences/index.html?inline=nyt-org>
showing
that language could significantly affect how quickly perceptions of color
are categorized. Russian and English speakers were asked look at three
blocks of color and say which two were the same.

Russian speakers must distinguish between lighter blues, or goluboy, and
darker blues, siniy, while English speakers do not have to, using only
"blue" for any shade. If the Russians were shown three blue squares with two
goluboy and one siniy, or the other way around, they picked the two matching
colors faster than if all three squares were shades from one blue group.
English makes no fundamental distinction between shades of blue, and English
speakers fared the same no matter the mix of shades.

In two different tests, subjects were asked to perform a nonverbal task at
the same time as the color-matching task. When the Russians simultaneously
carried out a nonverbal task, they kept their color-matching advantage. But
when they had to perform a verbal task at the same time as color-matching,
their advantage began to disappear. The slowdown suggested that the speed of
their reactions did not result just from a learned difference but that
language was actively involved in identifying colors as the test was
happening. Two other recent studies also demonstrated an effect of language
on color perception and provided a clue as to why previous experimental
results have been inconclusive. In The Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences, Dr. Paul Kay of the International Computer Science Institute at
Berkeley and colleagues hypothesized that if language is dominant on the
left side of the brain, it should affect color perception in the right
visual field. (The right visual field is connected to the left side of the
brain, and vice versa.)

English-speaking subjects were shown a ring of 12 small squares that were
all the same color except an odd one on the left or the right. If the odd
square was shown to the right visual field and it was from a completely
different color category in English, like a green square compared to the
ring of blue squares, then subjects were quick to identify it as different.
If the odd square shown to the right visual field was the same basic color
as the ring of squares, perhaps just being a different shade of blue,
subjects were not as fast to recognize the difference. If the odd square was
shown to the left visual field, it didn't matter if it was a different color
or only a different shade.

The extent to which language affected color perception depended on the side
of the brain being used.

Dr. Lupyan has also investigated how quickly the effects of language might
come into play. In one experiment, he asked students to look at a computer
screen that had "2" once and "5" many times in a circle. Over hundreds of
trials where the positions of the numbers changed, the students were asked
to "find the target" or "find the 'two.' "

Whenever subjects heard the word "two," they always found the numeral
faster. They found the "2" even faster when instructed to "ignore the 5s,"
as opposed to "ignore the distracters." In these cases, Dr. Lupyan
suggested, language is "greasing the wheels of perception."

Language also has a significant role in seeing and remembering where objects
are in space. Dr. Dedre Gentner at Northwestern and her colleagues conducted
experiments on the spatial reasoning of hearing children and children who
"home-sign."

Home-signers have hearing parents, but they are congenitally deaf and have
never been taught a sign language, according to Susan Goldin-Meadow, an
expert in homesign. The gestural language they develop is invented solely by
themselves. In the past, Dr. Gentner and her colleagues had observed that
children who home-sign did not appear to invent gestures for locations
spontaneously.

The children were shown two side-by-side boxes. Internally, each box was
divided in three. In each space was a card.

During each trial, the experimenter took a card from the first box and
showed the child that it had a special star on the back. Replacing it in the
first box in the same space, the experimenter asked the child to find where
the special card would be in the second box. Essentially, the children were
asked to map the position of the target card in the first box to the same
position in the second.

The researchers found that children without words for spatial relationships,
whether young or home-signers, had much more trouble finding the special
card in the second box than older hearing children who had learned the
relevant words.

For young hearing children, exposure to spatial language in the experiment
strongly influenced the success rate. If the experimenter used spatial terms
when speaking to a child, saying, "I'm putting the card in the top" (or
"middle" or "bottom"), as opposed to, "I'm putting the card here," the
children were much likelier to find the correct spot in the second box.

The effect lasted not just through the experiment, but until at least two
days later, when the children were retested.

"By giving us a framework for marshaling our thoughts, language does a lot
for us," Professor Gentner said. "Because spatial language gives us symbols
for spatial patterns, it helps us carve up the world in specific ways."

There is other evidence that a lack of spatial language is not a handicap in
solving spatial problems. In 2006, scientists published an experiment that
investigated the ability of the Amazonian Munduruku tribe to understand and
manipulate geometric relationships for which their language has no words.
The Munduruku performed about the same as Americans whose language is rich
with spatial terms.

This separation of language and thought is emphasized in a recent book
by Steven
Pinker<http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/people/p/steven_pinker/index.html?inline=nyt-per>,
at Harvard University<http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/organizations/h/harvard_university/index.html?inline=nyt-org>,
a skeptic of "neo-whorfianism." In "The Stuff of Thought: Language as a
Window Into Human Nature," Pinker explores the complicated ways that
language and thought relate to each other. He cautions against confusing the
"many ways in which language connects to thought." "Language surely affects
thought," he writes, but he argues that there is little evidence for the
claims that language can force people to have particular thoughts or make it
impossible for them to think in certain ways. With numbers, the importance
of language evidence is much clearer. It appears that the ability to count
is necessary to deal with large, specific numbers. And the only way to count
past a certain point is with language.

Elizabet Spaepen, a doctoral student at the University of
Chicago<http://topics.nytimes.com/top/reference/timestopics/organizations/u/university_of_chicago/index.html?inline=nyt-org>,
examined the ability of home-signing adults in Nicaragua to use numbers. Ms.
Spaepen emphasized that although the subjects had never been taught a formal
sign language, including counting, they were fully integrated in society.
They have jobs and they are paid as much as hearing or signing adults.

Ms. Spaepen asked the home-signers to match an array of objects laid out
before them. For example, she placed plastic discs on a table and encouraged
the subjects to lay out the same number of discs. If the number was small,
as in one, two or three, the home-signers got it right all the time.

If the number was larger, the home-signers got it right just approximately.
If Ms. Spaepen laid out four discs, the subjects might lay out five or six.
Although they were never quite right, they were never completely wrong. The
home-signers would not lay out one or 15 discs in response to four.

Scientists have shown that the understanding of small, specific numbers is a
trait with long evolutionary history. Monkeys and other animals can compute
the exact number of a small set of objects at a glance without explicitly
counting. The ability is called subitization.

Ms. Spaepen suggests that when home-signers correctly use small numbers,
they are relying on this innate trait. The count list we learn with most
languages (some languages do not have a count list or words for specific
numbers greater than three) has enabled humans to build on this heritage,
taking the specific and uniform gap between "one" and "two" and "two" and
"three," and extending it out through four and higher, theoretically to
infinity.

In another experiment, Dr. Lupyan showed subjects a series of chairs and
tables using pictures from the Ikea catalog. Some subjects were asked to
press a button indicating that the picture was of a table or a chair. Other
subjects pressed a button to make a nonverbal judgment about the pictures,
for example, to indicate whether they liked them or not. Dr. Lupyan found
that the subjects who used words to label the objects had more trouble
remembering whether they'd seen a specific chair before than subjects who
had only pressed a button in a nonverbal task.

Language helps us learn novel categories, and it licenses our unusual
ability to operate on an abstract plane, Dr. Lupyan said. The problem is
that after a category has been learned, it can distort the memory of
specific objects, getting between us and the rest of the nonabstract world.

Copyright 2008 <http://www.nytimes.com/ref/membercenter/help/copyright.html>
 The New York Times Company <http://www.nytco.com/>

   - Privacy Policy <http://www.nytimes.com/privacy>
   - Search <http://query.nytimes.com/search/advanced/>
   - Corrections <http://www.nytimes.com/corrections.html>
   - RSS <http://www.nytimes.com/rss>
   - First Look <http://firstlook.nytimes.com/>
   - Help <http://www.nytimes.com/membercenter/sitehelp.html>
   - Contact Us<http://nytimes.com/ref/membercenter/help/infoservdirectory.html>

   - Work for Us<https://careers.nytco.com/TAM/nyt_docs/TAM/candidate.html>

   - Site Map <http://spiderbites.nytimes.com/>
_______________________________________________
xmca mailing list
xmca@weber.ucsd.edu
http://dss.ucsd.edu/mailman/listinfo/xmca
Received on Wed Apr 23 14:14 PDT 2008

This archive was generated by hypermail 2.1.8 : Thu May 01 2008 - 17:14:14 PDT